dc.contributor.author | Waliński, Jacek Tadeusz | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2019-01-08T15:14:56Z | |
dc.date.available | 2019-01-08T15:14:56Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2018 | |
dc.identifier.citation | Waliński J.T., Verbs in Fictive Motion, WUŁ, Łódź 2018, doi: 10.18778/8142-382-3. | pl_PL |
dc.identifier.isbn | 978-83-8142-382-3 | |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/11089/26383 | |
dc.description.abstract | This book presents a corpus-based study of verbs used in expressions of fictive motion, which refers to the cognitive-linguistic phenomenon of describing material objects incapable of movement in terms of motion over their configuration in space. The study focuses specifically on the category of coextension paths, which are used to describe the form, orientation, or location of a spatially extended object in terms of a path over the object’s extent. The analysis, carried out using the British National Corpus, indicates that in English only a fraction of motion verbs are used consistently to express coextension paths, and that some of them are used for this purpose far more systematically than others. A holographic image of structuring coextension paths that emerges from the linguistic data indicates that whereas directional motion verbs tend to be used in fictive motion to express bounded paths, directions, and routes, verbs of motion manner are employed to specify shapes constituting subjective counterparts of spatial contours of actual motion. Moreover, depending on the particular use and the wider linguistic context, certain coextension path expressions can be interpreted as a result of conceptual blending, which fuses multiple facets of motion via a common communicative platform established dynamically in discourse. From the perspective of the analysis, these interpretations are not mutually irreconcilable. The evocation of a particular conceptualization triggered by the semantic attributes conflated in a verb and its satellites is likely to depend not only on individual comprehension strategies, but also on the degree of cultural-linguistic conventionalization of certain fictive motion patterns established through the processes of language acquisition and social transfer. | pl_PL |
dc.language.iso | en | pl_PL |
dc.publisher | Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego | pl_PL |
dc.rights | Uznanie autorstwa-Użycie niekomercyjne-Bez utworów zależnych 3.0 Polska | * |
dc.rights.uri | http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/pl/ | * |
dc.subject | motion in language | pl_PL |
dc.subject | cognitive linguistic models | pl_PL |
dc.subject | fictive motion in language | pl_PL |
dc.subject | British National Corpus | pl_PL |
dc.title | Verbs in Fictive Motion | pl_PL |
dc.type | Book | pl_PL |
dc.page.number | 278 | pl_PL |
dc.contributor.authorAffiliation | Uniwersytet Łódzki, Wydział Filologiczny, Instytut Anglistyki | pl_PL |
dc.identifier.eisbn | 978-83-8142-383-0 | |
dc.references | Alexander, H. G. (Ed.). (1956). The Leibniz-Clarke Correspondence: Together with Extracts from Newton’s Principia and Optiks. Manchester: Manchester University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Allan, K. (2009). An Inquest into Metaphor Death: Exploring the Loss of Literal Senses of Conceptual Metaphors. Cognitive Semiotics, 5(1–2). https://doi.org/10.1515/cogsem.2013.5.12.291 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Allan, K. (2013). What is Common Ground? In A. Capone, F. Lo Piparo, & M. Carapezza (Eds.), Perspectives on Linguistic Pragmatics (Vol. 2, pp. 285–310). Cham: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aloni, M., & Dekker, P. J. E. (Eds.). (2016). The Cambridge Handbook of Formal Semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Altmann, G. T. M. (2004). Language-mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: The ‘blank screen paradigm.’ Cognition, 93(2), B79–B87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.02.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (2007). The real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: Linking anticipatory (and other) eye movements to linguistic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(4), 502–518. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.12.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (2009). Discourse-mediation of the mapping between language and the visual world: Eye movements and mental representation. Cognition, 111(1), 55–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.12.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Amagawa, T. (1997). Subjective Motion in English and Japanese: A Case Study of Run and Hashiru. Tsukuba English Studies, 16, 33–50. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Anderson, S. E., & Spivey, M. J. (2009). The enactment of language: Decades of interactions between linguistic and motor processes. Language and Cognition, 1(01), 87–111. https://doi.org/10.1515/LANGCOG.2009.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aquinas, St. Thomas. (1269/1999). Commentary on Aristotle’s Metaphysics [written c. 1269]. (R. J. Blackwell, R. J. Spath, W. E. Thirkel, & P. H. Conway, Trans.). Notre Dame, IN: Dumb Ox Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aristotle. (350BC/1995a). On the Soul [written c. 350BC]. In J. Barnes (Ed.), The complete works of Aristotle (Vol. 1). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aristotle. (350BC/1995b). Physics [written c. 350BC]. In J. Barnes (Ed.), The complete works of Aristotle (Vol. 1). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aske, J. (1989). Path Predicates in English and Spanish: A Closer Look. In Proceedings of the Fifteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 1–14). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aston, G., & Burnard, L. (1998). The BNC Handbook: Exploring the British National Corpus with SARA. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Athanasiadou, A., Canakis, C., & Cornillie, B. (Eds.). (2006). Subjectification: Various Paths to Subjectivity. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Damasio, A. (2008). Embodied semantics for actions: Findings from functional brain imaging. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102(1–3), 35–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.012 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bączkowska, A. (2011). Space, Time & Language: A Cognitive Analysis of English Prepositions. Bydgoszcz: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Kazimierza Wielkiego. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Baker, R. K., Pettigrew, T. L., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2014). Infants’ ability to associate motion paths with object kinds. Infant Behavior and Development, 37(1), 119–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.12.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ballard, K. E. (1960). Leibniz’s Theory of Space and Time. Journal of the History of Ideas, 21(1), 49–65. https://doi.org/10.2307/2707998 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barlew, J. (2017). The semantics and pragmatics of perspectival expressions in English and Bulu: The case of deictic motion verbs (Ph.D. Dissertation). Ohio State University, Ohio. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barnes, J. (1982). The Presocratic Philosophers. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 577–660. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002149 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18(5–6), 513–562. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960344000026 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded Cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 617–645. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barsalou, L. W. (2009a). Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 1281–1289. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0319 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barsalou, L. W. (2009b). Situating concepts. In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition (pp. 236–263). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded Cognition: Past, Present, and Future. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(4), 716–724. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, W. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. In M. de Vega, A. M. Glenberg, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols and Embodiment: Debates on meaning and cognition (pp. 245–283). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Batki, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Connellan, J., & Ahluwalia, J. (2000). Is there an innate gaze module? Evidence from human neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 23(2), 223–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00037-6 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Beavers, J. (2008). Scalar complexity and the structure of events. In J. Dölling, T. Heyde-Zybatow, & M. Schäfer (Eds.), Event structures in linguistic form and interpretation (pp. 245–265). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Beavers, J., & Koontz-Garboden, A. (2012). Manner and Result in the Roots of Verbal Meaning. Linguistic Inquiry, 43(3), 331–369. https://doi.org/10.1162/LING_a_00093 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Beavers, J., Levin, B., & Wei Tham, S. (2010). The typology of motion expressions revisited. Journal of Linguistics, 46(02), 331–377. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226709990272 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bedny, M., & Caramazza, A. (2011). Perception, action, and word meanings in the human brain: The case from action verbs. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1224(1), 81–95. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06013.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Behnke, E. A. (2011). Husserl’s Phenomenology of Embodiment. In J. Fieser & B. Dowden (Eds.), Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved on 21.09.2014 from http://www.iep.utm.edu/husspemb/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bennardo, G. (2004). Linguistic untranslatability vs. conceptual nesting of frames of reference. In K. Forbus, D. Gentner, & T. Regier (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 102–107). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Benussi, V. (1913). Psychologie der Zeitauffassung. Heidelberg: Carl Winter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergen, B. K. (2012). Louder Than Words: The New Science of How the Mind Makes Meaning. New York: Basic Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergen, B. K. (2016). Embodiment, simulation and meaning. In N. Riemer (Ed.), The Routledge Handbook of Semantics (pp. 142–157). London: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergen, B. K., & Chang, N. (2013). Embodied Construction Grammar. In T. Hoffmann & G. Trousdale (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar (pp. 168–190). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergen, B. K., Chan Lau, T.-T., Narayan, S., Stojanovic, D., & Wheeler, K. (2010). Body part representations in verbal semantics. Memory & Cognition, 38(7), 969–981. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.7.969 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergen, B. K., Lindsay, S., Matlock, T., & Narayanan, S. (2007). Spatial and Linguistic Aspects of Visual Imagery in Sentence Comprehension. Cognitive Science, 31(5), 733–764. https://doi.org/10.1080/03640210701530748 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergson, H. (1922/2002). Duration and Simultaneity [First published in 1922 as Durée et simultanéité]. (L. Jacobson, Trans.). In Henri Bergson: Key Writings. (K. Ansell-Pearson & J. Mullarkey, Eds.). New York: Continuum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Berlin, B., & Kay, P. (1969). Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Biber, D. (1993). Representativeness in Corpus Design. Literary and Linguistic Computing, 8(4), 243–257. https://doi.org/10.1093/llc/8.4.243 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Biber, D., & Reppen, R. (Eds.). (2015). The Cambridge Handbook of English Corpus Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Biber, D., Conrad, S., & Reppen, R. (1998). Corpus Linguistics: Investigating Language Structure and Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G. N., Conrad, S., & Finegan, E. (1999). Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow: Pearson Longman. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). Where Is the Semantic System? A Critical Review and Meta-Analysis of 120 Functional Neuroimaging Studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 2767–2796. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp055 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Binnick, R. I. (1991). Time and the Verb: A Guide to Tense and Aspect. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Blake, F. R. (1930). A Semantic Analysis of Case. Language, 6(4), 34. https://doi.org/10.2307/521984 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Blomberg, J. (2014). Motion in Language and Experience: Actual and Non-actual motion in Swedish, French and Thai (Ph.D. Dissertation). Lund University, Lund. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Blomberg, J. (2015). The expression of non-actual motion in Swedish, French and Thai. Cognitive Linguistics, 26(4), 657–696. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2015-0025 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Blomberg, J., & Zlatev, J. (2014). Actual and non-actual motion: Why experientialist semantics needs phenomenology (and vice versa). Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 395–418. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-013-9299-x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bloom, P., & Keil, F. C. (2001). Thinking Through Language. Mind and Language, 16(4), 351–367. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00175 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bohnemeyer, J. (2003). The Unique Vector Constraint: The Impact of Direction Changes on the Linguistic Segmentation of Motion Events. In E. van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing Direction in Language and Space (pp. 86–110). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bohnemeyer, J. (2010). The Language-Specificity of Conceptual Structure: Path, Fictive Motion, and Time Relations. In B. Malt & P. Wolff (Eds.), Words and the Mind (pp. 111–137). Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bohnemeyer, J., Enfield, N. J., Essegbey, J., Ibarretxe-Antunano, I., Kita, S., Lüpke, F., & Ameka, F. K. (2007). Principles of event segmentation in language: The case of motion events. Language, 83(3), 495–532. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2007.0116 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00073-6 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does Language Shape Thought?: Mandarin and English Speakers’ Conceptions of Time. Cognitive Psychology, 43(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.2001.0748 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boroditsky, L. (2011). How Language Shapes Thought. Scientific American, 304(2), 62–65. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0211-62 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boroditsky, L., & Gaby, A. (2010). Remembrances of Times East: Absolute Spatial Representations of Time in an Australian Aboriginal Community. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1635–1639. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610386621 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M. (2002). The Roles of Body and Mind in Abstract Thought. Psychological Science, 13(2), 185–189. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00434 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bowdle, B. F., & Gentner, D. (2005). The Career of Metaphor. Psychological Review, 112(1), 193–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.193 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bower, G. H., & Morrow, D. G. (1990). Mental models in narrative comprehension. Science, 247(4938), 44–48. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2403694 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brandt, L. (2009). Subjectivity in the act of representing: The case for subjective motion and change. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 573–601. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-009-9123-9 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brewer, B., & Pears, J. (1993). Introduction: Frames of reference. In N. Eilan, R. A. McCarthy, & B. Brewer (Eds.), Spatial Representation: Problems in Philosophy and Psychology (pp. 25–30). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brezina, V. (2018). Statistics in Corpus Linguistics: A Practical Guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Britton, A., Setchi, R., & Marsh, A. (2013). Intuitive interaction with multifunctional mobile interfaces. Journal of King Saud University - Computer and Information Sciences, 25(2), 187–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jksuci.2012.11.002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Broccias, C., & Hollmann, W. B. (2007). Do we need summary and sequential scanning in (Cognitive) grammar? Cognitive Linguistics, 18(4), 487–522. https://doi.org/10.1515/COG.2007.026 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brown, A., & Gullberg, M. (2008). Bidirectional crosslinguistic influence in L1-L2 encoding of manner in speech and gesture: A Study of Japanese Speakers of English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30(2), 225–251. https://doi.org/10.10170/S0272263108080327 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brown, P. (2006). A sketch of the grammar of space in Tzeltal. In S. C. Levinson & D. P. Wilkins (Eds.), Grammars of Space: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity (pp. 230–272). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brunyé, T. T., Ditman, T., Mahoney, C. R., Augustyn, J. S., & Taylor, H. A. (2009). When You and I Share Perspectives: Pronouns Modulate Perspective Taking During Narrative Comprehension. Psychological Science, 20(1), 27–32. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02249.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bueti, D., & Walsh, V. (2009). The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1831–1840. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0028 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Burnard, L. (Ed.). (2000). Reference Guide for the British National Corpus (World Edition). Oxford: Oxford University Computing Services. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Burnett, P. (1978). Time Cognition and Urban Travel Behavior. Geografiska Annaler, Series B, Human Geography, 60(2), 107. https://doi.org/10.2307/490634 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bybee, J. L. (2006). From Usage to Grammar: The Mind’s Response to Repetition. Language, 82(4), 711–733. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2006.0186 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bybee, J. L. (2010). Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cacciari, C., Bolognini, N., Senna, I., Pellicciari, M. C., Miniussi, C., & Papagno, C. (2011). Literal, fictive and metaphorical motion sentences preserve the motion component of the verb: A TMS study. Brain and Language, 119(3), 149–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.05.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cadierno, T. (2008). Learning to talk about motion in a foreign language. In P. Robinson & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition (pp. 239–275). New York: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cadierno, T., & Robinson, P. (2009). Language typology, task complexity and the development of L2 lexicalization patterns for describing motion events. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 7, 245–276. https://doi.org/10.1075/arcl.7.10cad | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cann, R. (1993). Formal Semantics: An Introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cantlon, J. F., & Brannon, E. M. (2006). Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans. Psychological Science, 17(5), 401–406. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01719.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cappelle, B., & Declerck, R. (2005). Spatial and temporal boundedness in English motion events. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(6), 889–917. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2004.10.012 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cardillo, E. R., Watson, C. E., Schmidt, G. L., Kranjec, A., & Chatterjee, A. (2012). From novel to familiar: Tuning the brain for metaphors. NeuroImage, 59(4), 3212–3221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.079 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Carlson, G. N. (1984). Thematic roles and their role in semantic interpretation. Linguistics, 22(3), 259–280. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1984.22.3.259 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Carlson, L. A. (2010). Encoding Space in Spatial Language. In K. S. Mix, L. B. Smith, & M. Gasser (Eds.), The Spatial Foundations of Language and Cognition (pp. 157–183). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Carlson, L. A., Regier, T., & Covey, E. (2003). Defining Spatial Relations: Reconciling Axis and Vector Representations. In E. van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing Direction in Language and Space (pp. 111–131). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Casasanto, D. (2008). Who’s Afraid of the Big Bad Whorf? Crosslinguistic Differences in Temporal Language and Thought. In P. Indefrey & M. Gullberg (Eds.), Time to Speak: Cognitive and Neural Prerequisites for Time in Language (pp. 63–79). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Casasanto, D. (2010). Space for thinking. In V. Evans & P. A. Chilton (Eds.), Language, Cognition and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions (pp. 453–478). London: Equinox. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106(2), 579–593. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cermisoni, R., Actis-Grosso, R., Stucchi, N., & Antonelli, M. (2010). Space and Time in Benussi Tau Effect. Proceedings of the 26th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics, 26, 483–488. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chatterjee, A. (2010). Disembodying cognition. Language and Cognition, 2(01), 79–116. https://doi.org/10.1515/langcog.2010.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chipman, S. F. (Ed.). (2017). The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1962). A transformational approach to syntax. In A. A. Hill, (Ed.), Third Texas Conference on Problems of Linguistic Analysis in English: May 9-12 1958 (pp. 124–158). Austin,TX: University of Texas. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on Language. New York: Pantheon Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on Government and Binding: The Pisa Lectures. Dordrecht: Foris. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin, and Use. New York: Praeger. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cienki, A. J. (1997). Some Properties and Groupings of Image Schemas. In M. Verspoor, K. Yi, & E. Sweetser (Eds.), Lexical and Syntactical Constructions and the Construction of Meaning (pp. 3–15). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cienki, A. J. (1998). STRAIGHT: An image schema and its metaphorical extensions. Cognitive Linguistics, 9(2), 107–150. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1998.9.2.107 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Clark, A. (2006). Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(8), 370–374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.06.012 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Clark, E. V. (1974). Normal States and Evaluative Viewpoints. Language, 50(2), 316–332. https://doi.org/10.2307/412440 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Clark, E. V., & Clark, H. H. (1979). When Nouns Surface as Verbs. Language, 55(4), 767–811. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Clark, H. H. (1996). Using Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Clausner, T. C. (2005). Image schema paradoxes: Implications for cognitive semantics. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 93–110). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Clausner, T. C., & Croft, W. (1999). Domains and image schemas. Cognitive Linguistics, 10(1), 1–31. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1999.001 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cohen, J., Hansel, C. E. M., & Sylvester, J. D. (1953). A New Phenomenon in Time Judgment. Nature, 172(4385), 901–901. https://doi.org/10.1038/172901a0 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cohen, J., Hansel, C. E. M., & Sylvester, J. D. (1955). Interdependence in judgments of space, time and movement. Acta Psychologica, 11, 360–372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(55)80098-4 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cohn, A. G., Bennett, B., Gooday, J., & Gotts, N. M. (1997). Qualitative Spatial Representation and Reasoning with the Region Connection Calculus. GeoInformatica, 1(3), 275–316. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009712514511 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Comrie, B. (1976). Aspect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Connell, L. (2007). Representing object colour in language comprehension. Cognition, 102(3), 476–485. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.02.009 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2009). Is a bear white in the woods? Parallel representation of implied object color during language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(3), 573–577. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.3.573 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Conway, L. G., Repke, M. A., & Houck, S. C. (2016). Psychological Spacetime: Implications of Relativity Theory for Time Perception. SAGE Open, 6(4), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244016674511 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Coulson, S. (2001). Semantic leaps: Frame-shifting and conceptual blending in meaning construction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Coulson, S., & Oakley, T. (2000). Blending basics. Cognitive Linguistics, 11(3–4), 175–196. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2001.014 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Coulson, S., & Oakley, T. (2005). Blending and coded meaning: Literal and figurative meaning in cognitive semantics. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(10), 1510–1536. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2004.09.010 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Coulson, S., & Van Petten, C. (2007). A special role for the right hemisphere in metaphor comprehension? ERP evidence from hemifield presentation. Brain Research, 1146, 128–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.03.008 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Coventry, K. R., Lynott, D., Cangelosi, A., Monrouxe, L., Joyce, D., & Richardson, D. C. (2010). Spatial language, visual attention, and perceptual simulation. Brain and Language, 112(3), 202–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2009.06.001 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Croft, W. (2003). Typology and Universals, 2nd Ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Croft, W., Barðdal, J., Hollmann, W. B., Sotirova, V., & Taoka, C. (2010). Revising Talmy’s typological classification of complex event constructions. In H. C. Boas (Ed.), Contrastive Studies in Construction Grammar (pp. 201–235). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Croft, W., & Cruse, D. A. (2004). Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dąbrowska, E., & Divjak, D. (Eds.). (2015). Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dancygier, B. (Ed.). (2017). The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dancygier, B., & Sweetser, E. (2014). Figurative language. New York: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Davidse, K., Vandelanotte, L., & Cuyckens, H. (Eds.). (2010). Subjectification, Intersubjectification and Grammaticalization. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Davidson, D. (1963/2001a). Actions, Reasons, and Causes [First published in 1963]. In Essays on Actions and Events, 2nd Ed. (pp. 3–19). Oxford: Clarendon Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Davidson, D. (1967/2001b). The Logical Form of Action Sentences [First published in 1967]. In Essays on Actions and Events, 2nd Ed. (pp. 105–122). Oxford: Clarendon Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Davidson, D. (1971/2001c). Agency [First published in 1971]. In Essays on Actions and Events, 2nd Ed. (pp. 43–62). Oxford: Clarendon Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Davies, M. (2009). The 385+ million word Corpus of Contemporary American English (1990–2008+): Design, architecture, and linguistic insights. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 14(2), 159–190. https://doi.org/10.1075/ijcl.14.2.02dav | pl_PL |
dc.references | Davies, M. (2010). The Corpus of Contemporary American English as the first reliable monitor corpus of English. Literary and Linguistic Computing, 25(4), 447–464. https://doi.org/10.1093/llc/fqq018 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Davies, M. (2015). Corpora: An introduction. In D. Biber & R. Reppen (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of English Corpus Linguistics (pp. 11–31). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | De Smedt, H., & Verstraete, J.-C. (2006). Coming to terms with subjectivity. Cognitive Linguistics, 17(3), 365–392. https://doi.org/10.1515/COG.2006.011 | pl_PL |
dc.references | De Smedt, J., & De Cruz, H. (2011). The role of material culture in human time representation: Calendrical systems as extensions of mental time travel. Adaptive Behavior, 19(1), 63–76. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059712310396382 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Declerck, R. (2007). Distinguishing between the aspectual categories “(a)telic”, “(im)-perfective” and “(non)bounded.” Kansas Working Papers in Linguistics, 29, 48–64. https://doi.org/10.17161/KWPL.1808.1787 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Deignan, A. (2005). Metaphor and Corpus Linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Deignan, A. (2008). Corpus Linguistics and Metaphor. In R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought (pp. 280–294). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Demonte, V., & McNally, L. (Eds.). (2012). Telicity, Change, and State: A cross-categorial view of event structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Depraetere, I. (1995). On the necessity of distinguishing between (un)boundedness and (a)telicity. Linguistics and Philosophy, 18(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00984959 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Deroy, O. (Ed.). (2017). Sensory Blending: On Synaesthesia and Related Phenomena. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Desagulier, G. (2017). Corpus Linguistics and Statistics with R: Introduction to Quantitative Methods in Linguistics. New York: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Desai, R. H., Conant, L. L., Binder, J. R., Park, H., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2013). A piece of the action: Modulation of sensory-motor regions by action idioms and metaphors. NeuroImage, 83, 862–869. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.044 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Descartes, R. (1637/1985a). Discourse on the Method. In J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, & D. Murdoch (Trans.), The Philosophical Writings of Descartes (pp. 111–151). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Descartes, R. (1644/1985b). Principles of Philosophy. In J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, & D. Murdoch (Trans.), The Philosophical Writings of Descartes (pp. 177–292). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Deutscher, G. (2010). Through the Language Glass: Why the World Looks Different in Other Languages. New York: Metropolitan Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dewell, R. B. (1994). Over again: Image-schema transformations in semantic analysis. Cognitive Linguistics, 5(4), 351–380. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1994.5.4.351 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dewell, R. B. (2005). Dynamic patterns of CONTAINMENT. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 369–393). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dirven, R. (1993). Dividing up physical and mental space into conceptual categories by means of English prepositions. In C. Zelinsky-Wibbelt (Ed.), The Semantics of Prepositions: From Mental Processing to Natural Language Processing (pp. 73–97). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dirven, R., & Verspoor, M. (Eds.). (2004). Cognitive Exploration of Language and Linguistics, Second Revised Edition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | DiSalle, R. (2006). Understanding Space-time: The Philosophical Development of Physics from Newton to Einstein. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | DiSalle, R. (2016). Space and Time: Inertial Frames. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2016). Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved on 02.12.2017 from https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2016/entries/spacetime-iframes/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ditman, T., Brunyé, T. T., Mahoney, C. R., & Taylor, H. A. (2010). Simulating an enactment effect: Pronouns guide action simulation during narrative comprehension. Cognition, 115(1), 172–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.014 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dodge, E., & Lakoff, G. (2005). Image schemas: From linguistic analysis to neural grounding. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 57–91). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dove, G. (2009). Beyond perceptual symbols: A call for representational pluralism. Cognition, 110(3), 412–431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.016 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dowden, B. (2009). Zeno’s Paradoxes. In J. Fieser & B. Dowden (Eds.), Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved on 11.09.2013 from https://www.iep.utm.edu/zeno-par/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dowty, D. R. (1979/1991a). Word Meaning and Montague Grammar: The Semantics of Verbs and Times in Generative Semantics and in Montague’s PTQ [First published in 1979]. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dowty, D. R. (1991b). Thematic proto-roles and argument selection. Language, 67(3), 547–619. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1991.0021 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Duchowski, A. T. (2007). Eye Tracking Methodology: Theory and Practice, 2nd Ed. London: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Duff, A. (1981). The Third Language: Recurrent Problems of Translation into English. New York: Pergamon Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Earman, J. (1992). World Enough and Space-Time: Absolute Versus Relational Theories of Space and Time. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Einstein, A. (1905/1952a). On the Electrodynamics of Moving Bodies [First published in 1905 as Zur Elektrodynamik bewegter Körper]. In W. Perrett & G. B. Jeffery (Trans.), The Principle of Relativity: A Collection of Original Papers on the Special and General Theory of Relativity (pp. 35–65). New York: Dover. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Einstein, A. (1916/1952b). The Foundation of the General Theory of Relativity. [First published in 1916 as Die Grundlage der allgemeinen Relativitätstheorie]. In W. Perrett & G. B. Jeffery (Trans.), The Principle of Relativity: A Collection of Original Papers on the Special and General Theory of Relativity (pp. 109–164). New York: Dover. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Engberg-Pedersen, E. (1999). Space and Time. In J. Allwood & P. Gärdenfors (Eds.), Cognitive Semantics: Meaning and Cognition (pp. 131–152). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Engelbert, M., & Carruthers, P. (2010). Introspection. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(2), 245–253. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.4 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Eschenbach, C. (2005). Contextual, Functional, and Geometric Components in the Semantics of Projective Terms. In L. A. Carlson & E. van der Zee (Eds.), Functional Features in Language and Space (pp. 71–91). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2003). The Structure of Time: Language, meaning and temporal cognition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2009). How Words Mean: Lexical Concepts, Cognitive Models, and Meaning Construction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2012). Cognitive linguistics. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(2), 129–141. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1163 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2013). Language and Time: A Cognitive Linguistics Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2014). The Language Myth: Why Language Is Not an Instinct. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2017). Cognitive Linguistics. In S. E. F. Chipman (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Science (pp. 283–300). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V., & Green, M. (2006). Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fabiszak, M. (2008). Corpus frequency as a guide to metaphor labelling. In Z. Wąsik & T. Komendziński (Eds.), Metaphor and Cognition (pp. 149–162). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fabiszak, M., & Konat, B. (2013). Zastosowanie korpusów językowych w języko-znawstwie kognitywnym. In P. Stalmaszczyk (Ed.), Metodologie językoznawstwa. Ewolucja języka. Ewolucja teorii językoznawczych. (pp. 131–142). Łódź: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G. (1985/1994). Mental Spaces: Aspects of Meaning Construction in Natural Language [First published in 1985 by MIT Press]. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G. (1997). Mappings in Thought and Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G. (2007). Mental Spaces. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 351–376). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G., & Lakoff, G. (2009). On Metaphor and Blending. Cognitive Semiotics, 5(1–2). https://doi.org/10.1515/cogsem.2013.5.12.393 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (1998). Conceptual Integration Networks. Cognitive Science, 22(2), 133–187. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2202_1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2000). Compression and global insight. Cognitive Linguistics, 11(3–4), 283–304. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2001.017 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2002). The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind’s Hidden Complexities. New York: Basic Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2008). Rethinking metaphor. In R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought (pp. 53–66). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fellbaum, C. (1990). English Verbs as a Semantic Net. International Journal of Lexicography, 3(4), 278–301. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijl/3.4.278 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fellbaum, C. (Ed.). (1998). WordNet: An Electronic Lexical Database. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fellbaum, C. (1999). The Organization of Verbs and Verb Concepts in a Semantic Net. In P. Saint-Dizier (Ed.), Predicative Forms in Natural Language and in Lexical Knowledge Bases (pp. 93–109). Dordrecht: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-2746-4_3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fellbaum, C. (2006). WordNet(s). In K. Brown (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics, Second Edition (pp. 665–670). Oxford: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B0-08-044854-2/00946-9 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fellbaum, C. (2017). WordNet: An Electronic Lexical Resource. In S. E. F. Chipman (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Science (pp. 301–314). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Filipović, L. (2007). Talking About Motion: A Crosslinguistic Investigation of Lexicalization Patterns. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fillmore, C. J. (1968). The case for case. In E. Bach & R. T. Harms (Eds.), Universals in Linguistic Theory (pp. 1–88). New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fillmore, C. J. (1975/1997). Lectures on Deixis [First published in 1975 as Santa Cruz Lectures on Deixis 1971]. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fillmore, C. J. (1982). Towards a Descriptive Framework for Spatial Deixis. In R. J. Jarvella & W. Klein (Eds.), Speech, Place, and Action: Studies of Deixis and Related Topics (pp. 31–59). New York: John Wiley. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fillmore, C. J. (1983). How to know whether you’re coming or going. In G. Rauh (Ed.), Essays on deixis (pp. 219–227). Tübingen: Gunter Narr. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fillmore, C. J. (1985). Frames and the semantics of understanding. Quaderni Di Semantica, 6(2), 222–254. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fillmore, C. J. (1992). “Corpus linguistics” or “Computer-aided armchair linguistics.” In J. Svartvik (Ed.), Directions in Corpus Linguistics: Proceedings of Nobel Symposium 82, Stockholm, 4-8 August 1991 (pp. 35–60). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fillmore, C. J., & Atkins, B. T. S. (2000). Describing polysemy: The case of “crawl.” In Y. Ravin & C. Leacock (Eds.), Polysemy: Theoretical and Computational Approaches (pp. 91–110). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fischer, K. (2010). Quantitative Methods in Cognitive Semantics. In D. Glynn & K. Fischer (Eds.), Quantitative Methods in Cognitive Semantics: Corpus-Driven Approaches (pp. 43–59). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fitch, W. T., Hauser, M. D., & Chomsky, N. (2005). The evolution of the language faculty: Clarifications and implications. Cognition, 97(2), 179–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fodor, J. A. (1975). The Language of Thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fodor, J. A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fodor, J. A. (2008). LOT 2: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford: Clarendon Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Foglia, L., & Wilson, R. A. (2013). Embodied cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4(3), 319–325. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1226 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Frankish, K., & Ramsey, W. (Eds.). (2012). The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Frawley, W. (1992). Linguistic Semantics. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Frege, G. (1892/1960). On Sense and Reference [Originally published as Über Sinn und Bedeutung in 1892]. In P. Geach & M. Black (Eds.), Translations from the Philosophical Writings of Gottlob Frege, 2nd Ed. (pp. 56–78). Oxford: Basil Blackwell. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Frege, G. (1918/1956). The Thought: A Logical Inquiry [Originally published as Der Gedanke in 1918]. Mind, 65(1), 289–311. https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/65.1.289 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gaby, A. (2012). The Thaayorre think of time like they talk of space. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 300. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00300 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Galton, A. (2011). Time flies but space does not: Limits to the spatialisation of time. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(3), 695–703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2010.07.002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gamborotto, P., & Muller, P. (2003). Ontological Problems for the Semantics of Spatial Expressions in Natural Language. In E. van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing Direction in Language and Space (pp. 144–165). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ganis, G., & Schendan, H. E. (2011). Visual imagery. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(3), 239–252. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.103 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Garside, R., Leech, G. N., & McEnery, T. (1997). Corpus Annotation: Linguistic Information from Computer Text Corpora. London: Longman. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Geeraerts, D. (2006). Methodology in Cognitive Linguistics. In G. Kristiansen, M. Achard, R. Dirven, & F. J. Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez (Eds.), Cognitive Linguistics: Current Applications and Future Perspectives (pp. 21–49). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Geeraerts, D. (2010). The doctor and the semantician. In D. Glynn & K. Fischer (Eds.), Quantitative Methods in Cognitive Semantics: Corpus-Driven Approaches (pp. 63–78). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Geeraerts, D., & Cuyckens, H. (Eds.). (2007). The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gehrke, B. (2008). Ps in Motion: On the semantics and syntax of P elements and motion events (Ph.D. Dissertation). Utrecht University, Utrecht. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gennari, S. P., Sloman, S. A., Malt, B. C., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). Motion events in language and cognition. Cognition, 83(1), 49–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00166-4 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gentner, D., & Bowdle, B. F. (2001). Convention, Form, and Figurative Language Processing. Metaphor and Symbol, 16(3–4), 223–247. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2001.9678896 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (Eds.). (2003). Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Geuder, W., & Weisgerber, M. (2008, March). Manner of Movement and the Conceptualization of Force. Slides presented at the Journée d’étude “Il y a manière et manière,” Université d’Artois, Arras. Retrieved on 22.05.2018 from https://semanticsarchive.net/Archive/Tk5YmEwN/MannerMovement_slidescompact.pdf | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibbs, R. W. (1994). The Poetics of Mind: Figurative Thought, Language, and Understanding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibbs, R. W. (2005). Embodiment and Cognitive Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibbs, R. W. (2007). Idioms and Formulaic Language. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 697–725). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibbs, R. W. (2017). Metaphor Wars: Conceptual Metaphors in Human Life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibbs, R. W., & Matlock, T. (2008). Metaphor, imagination, and simulation. In R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought (pp. 161–176). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gijssels, T., & Casasanto, D. (2017). Conceptualizing Time in Terms of Space: Experimental Evidence. In B. Dancygier (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 651–668). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gill, M. L., & Lennox, J. G. (Eds.). (1994). Self-Motion: From Aristotle to Newton. Princeton, N.J: Princeton University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Giora, R. (1997). Understanding figurative and literal language: The graded salience hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics, 8(3), 183–206. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1997.8.3.183 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Giora, R. (2003). On Our Mind: Salience, Context, and Figurative Language. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glebkin, V. (2015). Is conceptual blending the key to the mystery of human evolution and cognition? Cognitive Linguistics, 26(1). https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2014-0067 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gleitman, L., & Papafragou, A. (2012). New Perspectives on Language and Thought. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning (pp. 543–568). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gleitman, L., & Papafragou, A. (2013). Relations Between Language and Thought. In D. Reisberg (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology (pp. 504–523). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20(1), 1–55. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glenberg, A. M. (2010). Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(4), 586–596. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.55 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glenberg, A. M., & Gallese, V. (2012). Action-based language: A theory of language acquisition, comprehension, and production. Cortex, 48(7), 905–922. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.010 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 558–565. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196313 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (1999). Indexical understanding of instructions. Discourse Processes, 28(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539909545067 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (2000). Symbol Grounding and Meaning: A Comparison of High-Dimensional and Embodied Theories of Meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(3), 379–401. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2714 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glucksberg, S. (2001). Understanding Figurative Language: From Metaphors to Idioms. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glucksberg, S. (2003). The Psycholinguistics of Metaphor. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 92–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00040-2 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glynn, D. (2010). Corpus-driven Cognitive Semantics: Introduction to the field. In D. Glynn & K. Fischer (Eds.), Quantitative Methods in Cognitive Semantics: Corpus-Driven Approaches (pp. 1–41). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glynn, D., & Fischer, K. (Eds.). (2010). Quantitative Methods in Cognitive Semantics: Corpus-Driven Approaches. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glynn, D., & Robinson, J. A. (Eds.). (2014). Corpus Methods for Semantics: Quantitative studies in polysemy and synonymy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Głaz, A. (2014). When -ities collide: Virtuality, actuality, reality. In K. Rudnicka-Szozda & A. Szwedek (Eds.), Cognitive Linguistics in the Making (pp. 77–88). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goddard, C. (1997). The semantics of coming and going. Pragmatics, 7(2), 147–162. https://doi.org/10.1075/prag.7.2.02god | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goddard, C. (Ed.). (2008). Cross-Linguistic Semantics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goddard, C. (2011). Semantic Analysis: A Practical Introduction (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goddard, C., & Wierzbicka, A. (2014). Words and meanings: Lexical semantics across domains, languages and cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goddard, C., Wierzbicka, A., & Wong, J. (2017). “Walking” and “running” in English and German: The conceptual semantics of verbs of human locomotion. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 14(2), 303–336. https://doi.org/10.1075/rcl.14.2.03god | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goldberg, A. E. (2003). Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(5), 219–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00080-9 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goldberg, A. E. (2010). Verbs, Constructions, and Semantic Frames. In M. Rappaport Hovav, E. Doron, & I. Sichel (Eds.), Lexical Semantics, Syntax, and Event Structure (pp. 39–58). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goldberg, A. E., & Suttle, L. (2010). Construction grammar. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(4), 468–477. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.22 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goschler, J., & Stefanowitsch, A. (Eds.). (2013). Variation and Change in the Encoding of Motion Events. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grady, J. E. (2005). Image schemas and perception: Refining a definition. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 35–55). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grady, J. E. (2007). Metaphor. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 188–213). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grant, E. (1981). Much Ado About Nothing: Theories of Space and Vacuum from the Middle Ages to the Scientific Revolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the Way of Words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T. (2009). What is Corpus Linguistics? Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(5), 1225–1241. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2009.00149.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T. (2012). Corpus linguistics, theoretical linguistics, and cognitive/psycholinguistics: Towards more and more fruitful exchanges. In J. Mukherjee & M. Huber (Eds.), Corpus Linguistics and Variation in English: Theory and Description (pp. 41–63). Amsterdam: Rodopi. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T. (2013). Statistics for Linguistics with R: A Practical Introduction, 2nd Revised Edition. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T. (2015). Quantitative designs and statistical techniques. In D. Biber & R. Reppen (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of English Corpus Linguistics (pp. 50–72). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T. (2017). Corpus Approaches. In B. Dancygier (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 590–606). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T., & Berez, A. L. (2017). Linguistic Annotation in/for Corpus Linguistics. In N. Ide & J. Pustejovsky (Eds.), Handbook of Linguistic Annotation (pp. 379–409). Dordrecht: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T., & Divjak, D. (2010). Quantitative approaches in usage-based Cognitive Semantics: Myths, erroneous assumptions, and a proposal. In D. Glynn & K. Fischer (Eds.), Quantitative Methods in Cognitive Semantics: Corpus-Driven Approaches (pp. 333–353). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T., & Stefanowitsch, A. (Eds.). (2006). Corpora in Cognitive Linguistics: Corpus-Based Approaches to Syntax and Lexis. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grondelaers, S., Geeraerts, D., & Speelman, D. (2007). A case for a Cognitive corpus linguistics. In M. Gonzalez-Marquez, I. Mittelberg, S. Coulson, & M. J. Spivey (Eds.), Methods in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 149–169). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gruber, J. S. (1965). Studies in Lexical Relations (Ph.D. Dissertation). MIT, Cambridge, MA. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gruber, J. S. (1976). Lexical Structures in Syntax and Semantics. Amsterdam: North Holland. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gryl, A., Moulin, B., & Kettani, D. (2002). A conceptual model for representing verbal expressions used in route descriptions. In K. R. Coventry & P. Olivier (Eds.), Spatial Language: Cognitive and Computational Perspectives (pp. 19–42). Dordrecht: Kluwer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gumperz, J. J., & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.). (1996). Rethinking Linguistic Relativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Habel, C. (2005). Verbs and Directions: The Interaction of Geometry and Function in Determining Orientation. In L. A. Carlson & E. van der Zee (Eds.), Functional Features in Language and Space (pp. 93–112). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Haiman, J. (1980). Dictionaries and encyclopedias. Lingua, 50(4), 329–357. https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(80)90089-3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hale, K. L., & Keyser, S. J. (1993). On Argument Structure and the Lexical Expression of Syntactic Relations. In K. L. Hale & S. J. Keyser (Eds.), View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger (pp. 53–109). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hale, K. L., & Keyser, S. J. (2002). Prolegomenon to a Theory of Argument Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hampe, B. (Ed.). (2005a). From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hampe, B. (2005b). Image schemas in Cognitive Linguistics: Introduction. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 35–55). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hampe, B. (2005c). When down is not bad, and up not good enough: A usage-based assessment of the plus–minus parameter in image-schema theory. Cognitive Linguistics, 16(1), 81–112. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2005.16.1.81 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Harasawa, I. (1994). A pragmatic view of V-te-i-ru and V-te-ar-u. Journal of Pragmatics, 22(2), 169–197. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-2166(94)90066-3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hasko, V. (2010). The Role of Thinking for Speaking in Adult L2 Speech: The Case of (Non)unidirectionality Encoding by American Learners of Russian. In Z.-H. Han & T. Cadierno (Eds.), Linguistic Relativity in SLA: Thinking for Speaking (pp. 34–58). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The Faculty of Language: What Is It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.298.5598.1569 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hawking, S. (1988). A Brief History of Time: From the Big Bang to Black Holes. New York: Bantam Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Helson, H. (1930). The tau effect – an example of psychological relativity. Science, 71(1847), 536–537. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.71.1847.536 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Heylen, K., Tummers, J., & Geeraerts, D. (2008). Methodological issues in corpus-based Cognitive Linguistics. In G. Kristiansen & R. Dirven (Eds.), Cognitive Sociolinguistics Language Variation, Cultural Models, Social Systems (pp. 91–128). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Higginbotham, J., Pianesi, F., & Varzi, A. (2000). Speaking of Events. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hill, A. A. (Ed.). (1962). Third Texas Conference on Problems of Linguistic Analysis in English: May 9-12 1958. Austin,TX: University of Texas. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hoffmann, T., & Trousdale, G. (Eds.). (2013). The Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hohenstein, J., Eisenberg, A., & Naigles, L. (2006). Is he floating across or crossing afloat? Cross-influence of L1 and L2 in Spanish–English bilingual adults. Bilingualism, 9(03), 249. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728906002616 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(3), 495–514. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.3.495 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Huggett, N. (2018). Zeno’s Paradoxes. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2018). Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved on 23.04.2018 from https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2018/entries/paradox-zeno/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Huggett, N., & Hoefer, C. (2015). Absolute and Relational Theories of Space and Motion. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2015). Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved on 11.09.2017 from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2015/entries/spacetime-theories/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Husserl, E. (1939/1970). The Origin of Geometry. In D. Carr (Trans.), The Crisis of European Sciences and Transcendental Phenomenology: An Introduction to Phenomenological Philosophy [Original work first published in 1939] (pp. 353–378). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Husserl, E. (1948/1973). Experience and Judgment: Investigations in a Genealogy of Logic [First published in 1948 as Erfahrung und Urteil: Untersuchungen zur Genealogie der Logik]. (J. S. Churchill & K. Ameriks, Trans.). Evanston: Northwestern University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hutchins, E. (2005). Material anchors for conceptual blends. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(10), 1555–1577. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2004.06.008 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Huumo, T. (2005). How fictive dynamicity motivates aspect marking: The riddle of the Finnish quasi-resultative construction. Cognitive Linguistics, 16(1). https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2005.16.1.113 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Huumo, T. (2017). The grammar of temporal motion: A Cognitive Grammar account of motion metaphors of time. Cognitive Linguistics, 28(1), 1–43. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2016-0015 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hwang, J. D., & Palmer, M. (2015). Identification of caused motion constructions. In Proceedings of the Fourth Joint Conference on Lexical and Computational Semantics (*SEM 2015) (pp. 51–60). Denver, CO: Association for Computational Linguistics. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hwang, J. D., Palmer, M., & Zaenen, A. (2013). Representing paths of motion in VerbNet. In T. H. King & V. de Paiva (Eds.), From Quirky Case to Representing Space: Papers in Honor of Annie Zaenen. (pp. 155–166). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ikegami, Y. (1969). The Semological Structure of the English Verbs of Motion (Linguistic Automation Project). New Haven, CT: Yale University. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Iwata, S. (1996). Motion and Extent: Two Sides of the Same Coin. Studia Linguistica, 50(3), 256–282. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9582.1996.tb00351.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1972). Semantic Interpretation in Generative Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1983). Semantics and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1985). Multiple subcategorization and the theta-criterion: The case of climb. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 3(3), 271–295. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00154264 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the Computational Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1990). Semantic Structures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1991). Parts and boundaries. Cognition, 41(1–3), 9–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(91)90031-X | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1992). Languages of the Mind: Essays on Mental Representation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1996a). Conceptual semantics and cognitive linguistics. Cognitive Linguistics, 7(1), 93–129. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1996.7.1.93 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1996b). The Architecture of the Linguistic-Spatial Interface. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and Space (pp. 1–30). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1996c). The Proper Treatment of Measuring Out, Telicity, and Perhaps Even Quantification in English. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 14(2), 305–354. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00133686 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1997). The Architecture of the Language Faculty. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (2007). Language, Consciousness, Culture: Essays on Mental Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (2011). What is the human language faculty?: Two views. Language, 87(3), 586–624. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2011.0063 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (2012). Language as a source of evidence for theories of spatial representation. Perception, 41(9), 1128–1152. https://doi.org/10.1068/p7271 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R., & Aaron, D. (1991). Review of “More Than Cool Reason: A Field Guide to Poetic Metaphor.” Language, 67(2), 320–338. https://doi.org/10.2307/415109 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R., & Pinker, S. (2005). The nature of the language faculty and its implications for evolution of language (Reply to Fitch, Hauser, and Chomsky). Cognition, 97(2), 211–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.04.006 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Janda, L. A. (2015). Cognitive Linguistics in the Year 2015. Cognitive Semantics, 1(1), 131–154. https://doi.org/10.1163/23526416-00101005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Janiak, A. (2012). Kant’s Views on Space and Time. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2012). Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved on 22.07.2013 from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2012/entries/kant-spacetime/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jaszczolt, K. M. (2009). Representing Time: An Essay on Temporality as Modality. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Johnson, M. (1987). The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Johnson, M. (2007). The Meaning of the Body: Aesthetics of Human Understanding. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language, Inference, and Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1989). The Computer and the Mind: An Introduction to Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kadosh, R. C., & Gertner, L. (2011). Synesthesia: Gluing Together Time, Number, and Space. In S. Dehaene & E. M. Brannon (Eds.), Space, Time and Number in the Brain (pp. 123–132). London: Academic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. New York: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kalisz, R. (2001). Językoznawstwo kognitywne w świetle językoznawstwa funkcjonalnego [Cognitive Linguistics From the Perspective of Functional Linguistics]. Gdańsk: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Gdańskiego. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kant, I. (1768/1968). Concerning the Ultimate Foundation of the Differentiation of Regions in Space [First published in 1768]. In G. B. Kerferd & D. E. Walford (Trans.), Selected Pre-Critical Writings and Correspondence with Beck (pp. 36–43). Manchester: Manchester University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kant, I. (1787/2003). Critique of Pure Reason [First published in 1787 as Kritik der reinen Vernunft]. (N. Kemp Smith, Trans.). New York: Palgrave Macmillan. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kardela, H. (2006a). Koncepcja umysłu ucieleśnionego w kognitywizmie. In W. Dziarnowska & A. Klawiter (Eds.), Mózg i jego umysły (pp. 217–243). Poznań: Wydawnictwo Zysk i S-ka. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kardela, H. (2006b). Metodologia językoznawstwa kognitywnego. In P. Stalmaszczyk (Ed.), Metodologie językoznawstwa współczesnego: Podstawy teoretyczne (pp. 196–233). Łódź: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kaschak, M. P., & Glenberg, A. M. (2000). Constructing Meaning: The Role of Affordances and Grammatical Constructions in Sentence Comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(3), 508–529. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2705 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kaschak, M. P., Madden, C. J., Therriault, D. J., Yaxley, R. H., Aveyard, M. E., Blanchard, A. A., & Zwaan, R. A. (2005). Perception of motion affects language processing. Cognition, 94(3), B79–B89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.06.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Katz, J. J., & Fodor, J. A. (1963). The Structure of a Semantic Theory. Language, 39(2), 170. https://doi.org/10.2307/411200 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kay, P. (2005). Argument structure constructions and the argument-adjunct distinction. In M. Fried & H. C. Boas (Eds.), Grammatical Constructions: Back to the Roots (pp. 71–98). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kay, P. (2013). The Limits of (Construction) Grammar. In T. Hoffmann & G. Trousdale (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar (pp. 32–48). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kay, P., & Kempton, W. (1984). What Is the Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis? American Anthropologist, 86(1), 65–79. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1984.86.1.02a00050 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kennedy, C. (2001). Polar opposition and the ontology of “degrees.” Linguistics and Philosophy, 24, 33–70. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005668525906 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kennedy, C., & Levin, B. (2008). Measure of change: The adjectival core of degree achievements. In L. McNally & C. Kennedy (Eds.), Adjectives and Adverbs: Syntax, Semantics, and Discourse (pp. 156–182). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kennedy, J. M. (1997). How the Blind Draw. Scientific American, 276(1), 76–81. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0197-76 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kenny, A. (1963/2003). Action, Emotion, and Will [First published in 1963]. London: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kiefer, M., & Pulvermüller, F. (2012). Conceptual representations in mind and brain: Theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions. Cortex, 48(7), 805–825. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.006 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kiparsky, P. (1997). Remarks on Denominal Verbs. In A. Alsina, J. Bresnan, & P. Sells (Eds.), Complex Predicates (pp. 473–499). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Klippel, A., Tenbrink, T., & Montello, D. R. (2013). The role of structure and function in the conceptualization of direction. In M. Vulchanova & E. van der Zee (Eds.), Motion Encoding in Language and Space (pp. 102–119). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kipper, K., Korhonen, A., Ryant, N., & Palmer, M. (2008). A large-scale classification of English verbs. Language Resources and Evaluation, 42(1), 21–40. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10579-007-9048-2 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Korzybski, A. (1933/1995). Science and Sanity: An Introduction to Non-Aristotelian Systems and General Semantics, 5th Ed. [First published in 1933]. Brooklyn, New York: Institute of General Semantics. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kosman, A. L. (1969). Aristotle’s Definition of Motion. Phronesis, 14(1), 40–62. https://doi.org/10.1163/156852869X00037 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kosman, A. L. (1987). Aristotle’s Definition of Change. History of Philosophy Quarterly, 4(1), 3–16. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Mental images and the Brain. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22(3–4), 333–347. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290442000130 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 635–642. https://doi.org/10.1038/35090055 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2003). Mental imagery: Against the nihilistic hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 109–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00025-1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kövecses, Z. (2005). Metaphor in Culture: Universality and Variation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kövecses, Z. (2010). Metaphor: A Practical Introduction, 2nd Ed. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kövecses, Z. (2015). Where Metaphors Come from: Reconsidering Context in Metaphor. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kreitzer, A. (1997). Multiple levels of schematization: A study in the conceptualization of space. Cognitive Linguistics, 8(4), 291–326. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1997.8.4.291 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Krzeszowski, T. P. (1993). The axiological parameter in preconceptual image schemata. In R. A. Geiger & B. Rudzka-Ostyn (Eds.), Conceptualizations and Mental Processing in Language (pp. 307–329). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Krzeszowski, T. P. (1997). Angels and Devils in Hell: Elements of Axiology in Semantics. Warszawa: Energeia. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kučera, H., & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence: Brown University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Laeng, B., & Teodorescu, D.-S. (2002). Eye scanpaths during visual imagery reenact those of perception of the same visual scene. Cognitive Science, 26(2), 207–231. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0364-0213(01)00065-9 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G. (1987a). Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal About the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G. (1987b). The Death of Dead Metaphor. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 2(2), 143–147. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327868ms0202_5 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G. (1993). The contemporary theory of metaphor. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and Thought, 2nd Ed (pp. 202–251). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. E. (2000). Where Mathematics Comes from: How the Embodied Mind Brings Mathematics into Being. New York: Basic Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G., & Turner, M. (1989). More Than Cool Reason: A Field Guide to Poetic Metaphor. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakusta, L., & Landau, B. (2005). Starting at the end: The importance of goals in spatial language. Cognition, 96(1), 1–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.03.009 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakusta, L., & Landau, B. (2012). Language and Memory for Motion Events: Origins of the Asymmetry Between Source and Goal Paths. Cognitive Science, 36(3), 517–544. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01220.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Landau, B. (2003). Axes and Direction in Spatial Language and Spatial Cognition. In E. van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing Direction in Language and Space (pp. 18–38). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Landau, B., & Jackendoff, R. (1993). “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(02), 217–265. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00029733 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Landau, B., Dessalegn, B., & Goldberg, A. M. (2010). Language and space: Momentary interactions. In V. Evans & P. A. Chilton (Eds.), Language, Cognition and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions (pp. 51–78). London: Equinox. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (1986). Abstract Motion. In Proceedings of the Twelfth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 455–471). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Vol. 1: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (1990). Subjectification. Cognitive Linguistics, 1(1), 5–38. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1990.1.1.5 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (1991). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Vol. 2: Descriptive Application. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (1991/2002). Concept, Image, and Symbol: The Cognitive Basis of Grammar, 2nd Ed. [First edition published in 1991]. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (1999). Virtual reality. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences, 29(2), 77–103. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2005). Dynamicity, fictivity, and scanning: The imaginative basis of logic and linguistic meaning. In D. Pecher & R. A. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding Cognition: The Role of Perception and Action in Memory, Language, and Thinking (pp. 164–197). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2006). Subjectification, grammaticization, and conceptual archetypes. In A. Athanasiadou, C. Canakis, & B. Cornillie (Eds.), Subjectification: Various Paths to Subjectivity (pp. 17–40). Berlin: De Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2008a). Cognitive Grammar A Basic Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2008b). Cognitive Grammar as a basis for language instruction. In P. Robinson & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition (pp. 66–88). New York: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2008c). Sequential and summary scanning: A reply. Cognitive Linguistics, 19(4), 533–607. https://doi.org/10.1515/COGL.2008.022 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2009a). A dynamic view of usage and language acquisition. Cognitive Linguistics, 20(3). https://doi.org/10.1515/COGL.2009.027 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2009b). Gramatyka kognitywna: Wprowadzenie. (E. Tabakowska & others, Trans.). Kraków: TAiWPN Universitas. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2012). Linguistic manifestations of the space-time (dis)analogy. In L. Filipović & K. M. Jaszczolt (Eds.), Space and Time in Languages and Cultures: Language, culture, and cognition (pp. 191–215). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2013). Essentials of Cognitive Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2014). Culture and Cognition, Lexicon and Grammar. In M. Yamaguchi, D. Tay, & B. Blount (Eds.), Approaches to Language, Culture, and Cognition: the Intersection of Cognitive Linguistics and Linguistic Anthropology (pp. 27–49). London: Palgrave Macmillan. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2015). Construal. In E. Dąbrowska & D. Divjak (Eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 120–142). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Leech, G. N. (2004). Meaning and the English Verb, 3rd Ed. New York: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Leech, G. N. (2005). Adding Linguistic Annotation. In W. Martin (Ed.), Developing Linguistic Corpora: A Guide to Good Practice (pp. 17–29). Oxford: Oxbow Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Leibowitz, H. W., Guzy, L. T., Peterson, E., & Blake, P. T. (1993). Quantitative perceptual estimates: Verbal versus nonverbal retrieval techniques. Perception, 22(9), 1051–1060. https://doi.org/10.1068/p221051 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levelt, W. J. M. (1996). Perspective Taking and Ellipsis in Spatial Descriptions. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and Space (pp. 77–108). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levin, B. (1993). English Verb Classes and Alternations: A Preliminary Investigation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (1991). Wiping the slate clean: A lexical semantic exploration. Cognition, 41(1–3), 123–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(91)90034-2 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (1992). The lexical semantics of verbs of motion: The perspective from unaccusativity. In I. M. Roca (Ed.), Thematic Structure. Berlin: De Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (1994). A preliminary analysis of causative verbs in English. Lingua, 92, 35–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(94)90337-9 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (2005). Argument Realization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (2006). Constraints on the complexity of verb meaning and VP structure. In H.-M. Gärtner, S. Beck, R. Eckardt, R. Musan, & B. Stiebels (Eds.), In Between 40 and 60 Puzzles for Krifka. Berlin: ZAS. Retrieved on 06.03.2018 from http://www.zas.gwz-berlin.de/fileadmin/material/40-60-puzzles-for-krifka/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (2013). Lexicalized Meaning and Manner/Result Complementarity. In B. Arsenijević, B. Gehrke, & R. Marín (Eds.), Studies in the Composition and Decomposition of Event Predicates (Vol. 93, pp. 49–70). Dordrecht: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-5983-1_3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levin, B., & Rappaport Hovav, M. (2014). Manner and result: A view from clean. In R. Pensalfini, M. Turpin, & D. Guillemin (Eds.), Studies in Language Companion Series (Vol. 147, pp. 337–358). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1075/slcs.147.14lev | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levinson, S. C. (1996). Frames of Reference and Molyneux’s Question: Crosslinguistic Evidence. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and Space (pp. 109–169). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levinson, S. C. (2003). Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levinson, S. C., & Wilkins, D. P. (Eds.). (2006a). Grammars of Space: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levinson, S. C., & Wilkins, D. P. (2006b). Patterns in the data: Towards a semantic typology of spatial description. In S. C. Levinson & D. P. Wilkins (Eds.), Grammars of Space: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity (pp. 512–552). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levinson, S. C., & Wilkins, D. P. (2006c). The background to the study of the language of space. In S. C. Levinson & D. P. Wilkins (Eds.), Grammars of Space: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity (pp. 1–23). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (1987). Conceptual Analysis, Linguistic Meaning and Verbal Interaction. Acta Universitatis Lodziensis. Łódź: Łódź University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (1996). Cross-linguistic and language-specific aspects of semantic prosody. Language Sciences, 18(1–2), 153–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/0388-0001(96)00013-7 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2007). Polysemy, Prototypes, and Radial Categories. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 139–169). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (Ed.). (2008a). Asymmetric Events. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2008b). Czym jest język? Dzisiejsze kontrowersje w paradygmatach generatywnych i kognitywnych. In P. Stalmaszczyk (Ed.), Metodologie językoznawstwa: współczesne tendencje i kontrowersje (pp. 9–26). Kraków: Lexis. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2010). Re-conceptualization and the emergence of discourse-meaning as a theory of translation. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk & M. Thelen (Eds.), Meaning in Translation (pp. 105–148). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2011). Events as They Are. In P. Stalmaszczyk (Ed.), Turning Points in the Philosophy of Language and Linguistics (pp. 35–64). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2012). Approximative Spaces and the Tolerance Threshold in Communication. International Journal of Cognitive Linguistics, 2(2), 165–183. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2017). Partial Perception and Approximate Understanding. Research in Language, 15(2), 129–152. https://doi.org/10.1515/rela-2017-0009 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B., & Dziwirek, K. (Eds.). (2009). Studies in Cognitive Corpus Linguistics. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lichtenberk, F. (1991). Semantic Change and Heterosemy in Grammaticalization. Language, 67(3), 475–509. https://doi.org/10.2307/415035 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Liu, J., Shang, J., & Han, J. (2017). Phrase Mining from Massive Text and Its Applications. San Rafael, CA: Morgan & Claypool. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Locke, J. (1689/1995). An Essay Concerning Human Understanding [First published in 1689]. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Logan, G. D., & Sadler, D. D. (1996). A Computational Analysis of the Apprehension of Spatial Relations. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and Space (pp. 493–529). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Logothetis, N. K. (2008). What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature, 453(7197), 869–878. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06976 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Loucks, J., & Pederson, E. (2011). Linguistic and non-linguistic categorization of complex motion events. In J. Bohnemeyer & E. Pederson (Eds.), Event Representation in Language and Cognition (pp. 108–133). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lourenco, S. F., & Longo, M. R. (2010). General Magnitude Representation in Human Infants. Psychological Science, 21(6), 873–881. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610370158 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lourenco, S. F., & Longo, M. R. (2011). Origins and Development of Generalized Magnitude Representation. In S. Dehaene & E. M. Brannon (Eds.), Space, Time and Number in the Brain (pp. 225–244). Elsevier. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Louw, B. (1993). Irony in the Text or Insincerity in the Writer? — The Diagnostic Potential of Semantic Prosodies. In M. Baker, G. Francis, & E. Tognini-Bonelli (Eds.), Text and Technology: In Honour of John Sinclair (pp. 157–176). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/z.64.11lou | pl_PL |
dc.references | Louw, B., & Milojkovic, M. (2014). Semantic prosody. In P. Stockwell & S. Whiteley (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Stylistics (pp. 263–280). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lovejoy, A. O. (1936). The Great Chain of Being: A Study of the History of an Idea. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lucy, J. A. (1992a). Grammatical Categories and Cognition: A Case Study of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lucy, J. A. (1992b). Language Diversity and Thought: A Reformulation of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lucy, J. A. (1994). The role of semantic value in lexical comparison: Motion and position roots in Yucatec Maya. Linguistics, 32(4–5), 623–656. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1994.32.4-5.623 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lucy, J. A. (1997). Linguistic Relativity. Annual Review of Anthropology, 26(1), 291–312. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.26.1.291 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lynch, K. (1960). The Image of the City. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics (Vols. 1–2). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | MacEachren, A. M. (1980). Travel Time as the Basis of Cognitive Distance. The Professional Geographer, 32(1), 30–36. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0033-0124.1980.00030.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mach, E. (1883/2013). The Science of Mechanics: A Critical and Historical Exposition of its Principles [First German edition in 1883]. (T. J. McCormack, Trans.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102(1–3), 59–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Majid, A., Bowerman, M., Kita, S., Haun, D. B. M., & Levinson, S. C. (2004). Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(3), 108–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.003 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Malt, B. C., Ameel, E., Imai, M., Gennari, S. P., Saji, N., & Majid, A. (2014). Human locomotion in languages: Constraints on moving and meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 74, 107–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.08.003 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Malt, B. C., Gennari, S. P., & Imai, M. (2010). Lexicalization patterns and the world-to-words mapping. In B. C. Malt & P. Wolff (Eds.), Words and the Mind: How Words Capture Human Experience (pp. 29–57). New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Malt, B. C., Gennari, S. P., Imai, M., Ameel, E., Tsuda, N., & Majid, A. (2008). Talking About Walking: Biomechanics and the Language of Locomotion. Psychological Science, 19(3), 232–240. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02074.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Malt, B. C., & Wolff, P. (Eds.). (2010). Words and the Mind: How Words Capture Human Experience. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99(4), 587–604. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.4.587 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mandler, J. M. (2004). The Foundations of Mind: Origins of Conceptual Thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mandler, J. M. (2005). How to build a baby: III. Image schemas and the transition to verbal thought. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 137–165). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mandler, J. M. (2010). The spatial foundations of the conceptual system. Language and Cognition, 2(01), 21–44. https://doi.org/10.1515/langcog.2010.002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mandler, J. M. (2012). On the Spatial Foundations of the Conceptual System and Its Enrichment. Cognitive Science, 36(3), 421–451. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01241.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mandler, J. M., & Cánovas, C. P. (2014). On defining image schemas. Language and Cognition, 6(04), 510–532. https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2014.14 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mani, I., & Pustejovsky, J. (2012). Interpreting Motion: Grounded Representations for Spatial Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mani, I., Pustejovsky, J., & Gaizauskas, R. (Eds.). (2005). The Language of Time: A Reader. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mari, A. (2006). What do the notions of instrumentality and of manner have in common? In P. Saint-Dizier (Ed.), Syntax and Semantics of Prepositions (pp. 263–287). Dordrecht: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Marr, D. (1982/2010). Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information [First published in 1982]. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Marr, D., & Vaina, L. (1982). Representation and Recognition of the Movements of Shapes. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 214(1197), 501–524. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1982.0024 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Martin, A. (2007). The Representation of Object Concepts in the Brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 25–45. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190143 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Martin, A., & Caramazza, A. (2003). Neuropsychological and Neuroimaging Perspectives on Conceptual Knowledge: An Introduction. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3–6), 195–212. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290342000050 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Martínez-Losa, N. J. (2006). Towards a typology of fictive motion events: Review of existing proposals and presentation of new perspectives. Interlinguistica, 17, 562–569. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T. (2001). How real is fictive motion? (Ph.D. Dissertation). University of California, Santa Cruz. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T. (2004a). Fictive motion as cognitive simulation. Memory & Cognition, 32(8), 1389–1400. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206329 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T. (2004b). The conceptual motivation of fictive motion. In G. Radden & K.-U. Panther (Eds.), Studies in Linguistic Motivation (pp. 221–248). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T. (2006). Depicting fictive motion in drawings. In J. Luchjenbroers (Ed.), Cognitive Linguistics Investigations (pp. 67–85). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T. (2010). Abstract motion is no longer abstract. Language and Cognition, 2(2), 243–260. https://doi.org/10.1515/langcog.2010.010 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T. (2017). Metaphor, Simulation, and Fictive Motion. In B. Dancygier (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 477–490). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T., & Bergmann, T. (2015). Fictive motion. In E. Dąbrowska & D. Divjak (Eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 546–562). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T., & Richardson, D. C. (2004). Do eye movements go with fictive motion? In K. Forbus, D. Gentner, & T. Regier (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 909–914). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T., Holmes, K. J., Srinivasan, M., & Ramscar, M. (2011). Even Abstract Motion Influences the Understanding of Time. Metaphor and Symbol, 26(4), 260–271. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2011.609065 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T., Ramscar, M., & Boroditsky, L. (2003). The experiential basis of meaning. In R. Alterman & D. Kirsh (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 792–797). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T., Ramscar, M., & Boroditsky, L. (2004). The experiential basis of motion language. In A. S. da Silva, A. Torres, & M. Gonçalves (Eds.), Linguagem, Cultura e Cognição, ou a Linguística Cognitiva, Vol. 2 (pp. 43–57). Coimbra: Almedina. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T., Ramscar, M., & Boroditsky, L. (2005). On the Experiential Link Between Spatial and Temporal Language. Cognitive Science, 29(4), 655–664. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_17 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T., Ramscar, M., & Srinivasan, M. (2005). Even the Most Abstract Motion Influences Temporal Understanding. In B. G. Bara, L. W. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (p. 2527). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matsumoto, N. (2010). The Pragmatics of Multi-Verb Sequences: The Case of the Verb Go. Lodz Papers in Pragmatics, 6(1), 117–143. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10016-010-0007-9 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matsumoto, Y. (1996a). Subjective motion and English and Japanese verbs. Cognitive Linguistics, 7(2), 183–226. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1996.7.2.183 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matsumoto, Y. (1996b). Subjective-change expressions in Japanese and their cognitive and linguistic bases. In G. Fauconnier & E. Sweetser (Eds.), Spaces, Worlds, and Grammar (pp. 124–156). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matthews, W. J., & Meck, W. H. (2014). Time perception: The bad news and the good. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5(4), 429–446. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1298 | pl_PL |
dc.references | McCloud, S. (1994). Understanding Comics: The Invisible Art. New York: HarperPerennial. | pl_PL |
dc.references | McDonough, J. K. (2014). Leibniz’s Philosophy of Physics. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2014). Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved on 12.06.2017 from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2014/entries/leibniz-physics/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | McEnery, T., & Hardie, A. (2012). Corpus Linguistics: Method, Theory and Practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | McEnery, T., & Hardie, A. (2013). The History of Corpus Linguistics. In K. Allan (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of the History of Linguistics (pp. 727–746). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | McEnery, T., & Wilson, A. (2001). Corpus Linguistics: An Introduction, 2nd Ed. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | McEnery, T., Xiao, R., & Tono, Y. (2006). Corpus-Based Language Studies: An Advanced Resource Book. London: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | McGlone, M. S., & Harding, J. L. (1998). Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perscpective in temporal language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(5), 1211–1223. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.24.5.1211 | pl_PL |
dc.references | McWhorter, J. H. (2014). The Language Hoax: Why the World Looks the Same in Any Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Merritt, D. J., Casasanto, D., & Brannon, E. M. (2010). Do monkeys think in metaphors? Representations of space and time in monkeys and humans. Cognition, 117(2), 191–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.011 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Meteyard, L., Cuadrado, S. R., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012). Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex, 48(7), 788–804. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2010.11.002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Millar, S. (2008). Space and Sense. Hove: Psychology Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Miller, G. A. (1995). WordNet: A lexical database for English. Communications of the ACM, 38(11), 39–41. https://doi.org/10.1145/219717.219748 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Miller, G. A., & Fellbaum, C. (2007). WordNet then and now. Language Resources and Evaluation, 41(2), 209–214. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10579-007-9044-6 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Miller, G. A., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1976). Language and Perception. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Minkowski, H. (1908/1952). Space and Time [A translation of an address delivered at the 80th Assembly of German Natural Scientists and Physicians, at Cologne, September 21, 1908]. In W. Perrett & G. B. Jeffery (Trans.), The Principle of Relativity: A Collection of Original Papers on the Special and General Theory of Relativity (pp. 71–95). New York: Dover. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mishra, R. K., & Singh, N. (2010). Online Fictive Motion Understanding: An Eye-Movement Study With Hindi. Metaphor and Symbol, 25(3), 144–161. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2010.489393 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Montague, R. (1974). Formal Philosophy: Selected Papers of Richard Montague. (R. H. Thomason, Ed.). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Moon, R. (1998). Fixed Expressions and Idioms in English: A Corpus-Based Approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Moore, K. E. (2014). The Spatial Language of Time: Metaphor, Metonymy, and Frames of Reference. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Murphy, G. L. (1996). On metaphoric representation. Cognition, 60(2), 173–204. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(96)00711-1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Murphy, G. L. (1997). Reasons to doubt the present evidence for metaphoric representation. Cognition, 62(1), 99–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00725-1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Müller, C. (2008). Metaphors Dead and Alive, Sleeping and Waking: A Dynamic View. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Nakazawa, T. (2007). A Typology of the Ground of Deictic Motion Verbs As Path-Conflating Verbs: The Speaker, the Addressee, and Beyond. Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 43(2), 59–82. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10010-007-0014-3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Nakazawa, T. (2009). A Typology of the Path of Deictic Motion Verbs as Path-Conflating Verbs: The Entailment of Arrival and the Deictic Center. Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 45(3), 385–403. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10010-009-0022-6 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Newman, J. (Ed.). (2002). The Linguistics of Sitting, Standing, and Lying. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Newman, J., & Lin, J. (2007). The purposefulness of going: A corpus-linguistic study. In J. T. Waliński, K. Kredens, & S. Goźdź-Roszkowski (Eds.), Corpora and ICT in Language Studies (pp. 293–308). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Newman, J., & Rice, S. (2004). Patterns of usage for English SIT, STAND, and LIE: A cognitively-inspired exploration in corpus linguistics. Cognitive Linguistics, 15(3), 351–396. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2004.013 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Newton, I. (1687/1995). The Principia: Mathematical Principles of Natural Philosophy [First published in 1687 as Philosophiae Naturalis Principia Mathematica]. (A. Motte, Trans.). Amherst, N.Y: Prometheus Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Nida, E. A. (1975). A Componential Analysis of Meaning An Introduction to Semantic Structures. The Hague: Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Nikanne, U., & van der Zee, E. (2013). The lexical representation of path curvature in motion expressions: A three‐way path curvature distinction. In M. Vulchanova & E. van der Zee (Eds.), Motion Encoding in Language and Space (pp. 187–212). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Núñez, R. E., & Cooperrider, K. (2013). The tangle of space and time in human cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(5), 220–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.008 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Núñez, R. E., Cooperrider, K., Doan, D., & Wassmann, J. (2012). Contours of time: Topographic construals of past, present, and future in the Yupno valley of Papua New Guinea. Cognition, 124(1), 25–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.007 | pl_PL |
dc.references | O’Keefe, J. (1996). The Spatial Prepositions in English, Vector Grammar, and the Cognitive Map Theory. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and Space (pp. 277–316). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | O’Keefe, J. (2003). Vector Grammar, Places, and the Functional Role of the Spatial Prepositions in English. In E. van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing Direction in Language and Space (pp. 69–85). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Oakley, T. (2007). Image schemas. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 214–235). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Onozuka, H. (2012). Notes on Type I Subjective Motion Expressions in English. Australian Journal of Linguistics, 32(2), 239–257. https://doi.org/10.1080/07268602.2012.674472 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Oshima, D. Y. (2006). GO and COME Revisited: What Serves as a Reference Point? In Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 287–298). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. https://doi.org/10.3765/bls.v32i1.3466 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Overgaard, S. (2012). Visual perception and self-movement: Another look. In A. Foolen, U. M. Lüdtke, T. P. Racine, & J. Zlatev (Eds.), Moving Ourselves, Moving Others: Motion and emotion in intersubjectivity, consciousness and language (pp. 81–104). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Özçalişkan, Ş. (2013). Ways of crossing a spatial boundary in typologically distinct languages. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(02), 485–508. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716413000325 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Palmer, J. (2016). Parmenides. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2016). Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved on 20.02.2018 from https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2016/entries/parmenides/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Palmer, J. (2017). Zeno of Elea. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2017). Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved on 16.03.2018 from https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2017/entries/zeno-elea/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Palmer, M., Bonial, C., & Hwang, J. D. (2017). VerbNet: Capturing English Verb Behavior, Meaning, and Usage. In S. E. F. Chipman (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Science (pp. 315–336). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Papafragou, A. (2010). Source-Goal Asymmetries in Motion Representation: Implications for Language Production and Comprehension. Cognitive Science, 34(6), 1064–1092. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01107.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Papafragou, A., Hulbert, J., & Trueswell, J. (2008). Does language guide event perception? Evidence from eye movements. Cognition, 108(1), 155–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.02.007 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Papafragou, A., Massey, C., & Gleitman, L. (2002). Shake, rattle, ‘n’ roll: The representation of motion in language and cognition. Cognition, 84(2), 189–219. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00046-X | pl_PL |
dc.references | Papafragou, A., Massey, C., & Gleitman, L. (2006). When English proposes what Greek presupposes: The cross-linguistic encoding of motion events. Cognition, 98(3), B75–B87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.05.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Papafragou, A., & Selimis, S. (2010). Event categorisation and language: A cross-linguistic study of motion. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(2), 224–260. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960903017000 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Parsons, T. (1990). Events in the Semantics of English: A Study in Subatomic Semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pawelec, A. (2006). The death of metaphor. Studia Linguistica Universitatis Iagellonicae Cracoviensis, 123, 117–121. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pecher, D., & Zwaan, R. A. (Eds.). (2005). Grounding Cognition: The Role of Perception and Action in Memory, Language, and Thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pederson, E. (2017). Approaches to Motion Event Typology. In A. Y. Aikhenvald & R. M. W. Dixon (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Typology (pp. 574–598). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pęzik, P. (2015). Spokes – a Search and Exploration Service for Conversational Corpus Data. In Selected Papers from CLARIN 2014, 99–109 Electronic Conference Proceedings (pp. 99–109). Linköping University Electronic Press: Linköping University Electronic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Phelps, K. S., & Duman, S. (2012). Manipulating Manner: Semantic Representations of Human Locomotion Verbs in English and German. In N. Miyake, D. Peebles, & R. P. Cooper (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 857–862). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piaget, J. (1946/1969). The Child’s Conception of Time [First published in 1946 as Le développement de la notion de temps chez l’enfant]. (A. J. Pomerans, Trans.). New York: Basic Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piaget, J. (1946/1970). The Child’s Conception of Movement and Speed [First published in 1946 as Les notions de mouvement et de vitesse chez l’enfant]. (G. E. T. Holloway & M. J. Mackenzie, Trans.). New York: Basic Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piaget, J. (1979). Relations Between Psychology and Other Sciences. Annual Review of Psychology, 30(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.30.020179.000245 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1948/1956). The Child’s Conception of Space [First published in 1948 as La représentation de l’espace chez l’enfant]. (F. J. Langdon & J. L. Lunzer, Trans.). New York: Humanities Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., & Szeminska, A. (1948/1960). The Child’s Conception of Geometry [First published in 1948 as La géométrie spontanée de l’enfant]. (E. A. Lunzer, Trans.). New York: Basic Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piantadosi, S. T. (2014). Zipf’s word frequency law in natural language: A critical review and future directions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(5), 1112–1130. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0585-6 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piasecki, M., Broda, B., & Szpakowicz, S. (2009). A Wordnet from the Ground Up. Wrocław: Oficyna Wydawnicza Politechniki Wrocławskiej. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. New York: William Morrow. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pinker, S. (2007). The Stuff of Thought: Language as a Window into Human Nature. New York: Viking. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pinker, S., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: What’s special about it? Cognition, 95(2), 201–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piñón, C. J. (1993). Paths and their names. In K. Beals, G. Cooke, D. Kathman, S. Kita, K.-E. McCullough, & D. Testen (Eds.), Papers from the 29th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (pp. 287–303). Chicago: CLS. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Plato. (388BC/1997). Cratylus [written c. 388 BC]. In J. M. Cooper & D. S. Hutchinson (Eds.), Plato: Complete Works (pp. 101–156). Indianapolis, IN: Hackett. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Podhorodecka, J. (2007). Evaluative Metaphor: Extended meanings of English motion verbs. Kraków: Wydawnictwo Naukowe Akademii Pedagogicznej. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Poincaré, H. (1905/1958). The Value of Science [First published in 1905 as La Valeur de la Science]. (G. B. Halsted, Trans.). New York: Dover. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pourcel, S. (2005). Relativism in the linguistic representation and cognitive conceptualisation of motion events across verb-framed and satellite-framed languages (Ph.D. Dissertation). University of Durham, Durham. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pourcel, S. (2010). Motion: a conceptual typology. In V. Evans & P. A. Chilton (Eds.), Language, Cognition and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions (pp. 419–449). London: Equinox. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Powers, D. M. W. (1998). Applications and explanations of Zipf’s law. In D. M. W. Powers (Ed.), NeMLaP3/CoNLL98: New Methods in Language Processing and Computational Natural Language Learning (pp. 151–160). Stroudsburg, PA: Association for Computational Linguistics. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Price-Williams, D. R. (1954). The Kappa Effect. Nature, 173(4399), 363–364. https://doi.org/10.1038/173363a0 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Primus, B. (2016). Participant roles. In N. Riemer (Ed.), The Routledge Handbook of Semantics (pp. 403–418). London: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(7), 576–582. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1706 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pulvermüller, F., & Fadiga, L. (2010). Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(5), 351–360. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2811 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pustejovsky, J. (1991). The syntax of event structure. Cognition, 41(1–3), 47–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(91)90032-Y | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2002). Mental imagery: In search of a theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(2), 157–182. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2003). Return of the mental image: Are there really pictures in the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 113–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00003-2 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G. N., & Svartvik, J. (1985). A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. London: Longman. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Radden, G. (1996). Motion metaphorized: The case of coming and going. In E. H. Casad (Ed.), Cognitive Linguistics in the Redwoods (pp. 423–458). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Radden, G., & Dirven, R. (2007). Cognitive English Grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Radvansky, G. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2011). Event perception. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(6), 608–620. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.133 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Radvansky, G. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2014). Event cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ramachandran, V. S., & Anstis, S. M. (1986). The Perception of Apparent Motion. Scientific American, 254(6), 102–109. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0686-102 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ramscar, M., Matlock, T., & Boroditsky, L. (2010). Time, Motion, and Meaning: The Experiential Basis of Abstract Thought. In K. S. Mix, L. B. Smith, & M. Gasser (Eds.), The Spatial Foundations of Language and Cognition (pp. 67–82). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ramscar, M., Matlock, T., & Dye, M. (2010). Running down the clock: The role of expectation in our understanding of time and motion. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(5), 589–615. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960903381166 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Randell, D., Cui, Z., & Cohn, A. G. (1992). A spatial logic based on regions and connection. In Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Knowledge Representation and Reasoning (pp. 165–176). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Randell, D., Cui, Z., & Cohn, A. G. (1992). A spatial logic based on regions and connection. In Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Knowledge Representation and Reasoning (pp. 165–176). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rappaport Hovav, M. (2014). Lexical content and context: The causative alternation in English revisited. Lingua, 141, 8–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2013.09.006 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rappaport Hovav, M., Doron, E., & Sichel, I. (Eds.). (2010). Lexical Semantics, Syntax, and Event Structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rappaport Hovav, M., & Levin, B. (1998). Building Verb Meanings. In M. Butt & W. Geuder (Eds.), The Projection of Arguments: Lexical and Compositional Factors (pp. 97–134). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rappaport Hovav, M., & Levin, B. (2010). Reflections on manner/result complementarity. In M. Rappaport Hovav, E. Doron, & I. Sichel (Eds.), Lexical Semantics, Syntax, and Event Structure (pp. 21–38). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rauh, G. (1981). On coming and going in English and German. Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 13, 53–68. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rauh, G. (Ed.). (1983). Essays on deixis. Tübingen: Gunter Narr. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Reines, M. F., & Prinz, J. (2009). Reviving Whorf: The Return of Linguistic Relativity. Philosophy Compass, 4(6), 1022–1032. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1747-9991.2009.00260.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Resnik, P. (1996). Selectional constraints: An information-theoretic model and its computational realization. Cognition, 61(1–2), 127–159. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00722-6 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Richardson, D. C., Dale, R., & Spivey, M. J. (2007). Eye movements in language and cognition: A brief introduction. In M. Gonzalez-Marquez, I. Mittelberg, S. Coulson, & M. J. Spivey (Eds.), Methods in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 323–344). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Richardson, D. C., & Matlock, T. (2007). The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye movement study of fictive motion. Cognition, 102(1), 129–138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.12.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Richardson, D. C., Spivey, M. J., Barsalou, L. W., & McRae, K. (2003). Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs. Cognitive Science, 27(5), 767–780. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0364-0213(03)00064-8 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rinck, M. (2005). Spatial Situation Models. In P. Shah & A. Miyake (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Visuospatial Thinking (pp. 334–382). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Robinson, H. (2012). Dualism. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2012). Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved on 25.09.2014 from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2012/entries/dualism/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rojo, A., & Valenzuela, J. (2003). Fictive Motion in English and Spanish. International Journal of English Studies, 3(2), 125–151. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rojo, A., & Valenzuela, J. (2009). Fictive Motion in Spanish: Travellable, non-travellable and path-related manner information. In J. Valenzuela, A. Rojo, & C. Soriano (Eds.), Trends in Cognitive Linguistics: Theoretical and Applied Models (pp. 244–260). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Romero Lauro, L. J., Mattavelli, G., Papagno, C., & Tettamanti, M. (2013). She runs, the road runs, my mind runs, bad blood runs between us: Literal and figurative motion verbs: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 83, 361–371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.050 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of Categorisation. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and Categorisation (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rossini, P. M., & Rossi, S. (2007). Transcranial magnetic stimulation: Diagnostic, therapeutic, and research potential. Neurology, 68(7), 484–488. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000250268.13789.b2 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J., & Pérez Hernández, L. (2011). The Contemporary Theory of Metaphor: Myths, Developments and Challenges. Metaphor and Symbol, 26(3), 161–185. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2011.583189 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rynasiewicz, R. (2014a). Newton’s Scholium on Time, Space, Place and Motion. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2014). Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved on 15.08.2017 from http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/newton-stm/scholium.html | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rynasiewicz, R. (2014b). Newton’s Views on Space, Time, and Motion. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2014). Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved on 04.08.2015 from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2014/entries/newton-stm/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sachs, J. (1995). Aristotle’s Physics: A Guided Study. New Brunswick, N.J: Rutgers University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sachs, J. (2005). Aristotle: Motion and its Place in Nature. In J. Fieser & B. Dowden (Eds.), Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved on 24.04.2018 from https://www.iep.utm.edu/aris-mot/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sampaio, W., Sinha, C., & Sinha, V. D. S. (2009). Mixing and mapping: Motion, path, and manner in Amondawa. In J. Guo, E. Lieven, N. Budwig, S. Ervin-Tripp, K. Nakamura, & Ş. Özçalişkan (Eds.), Crosslinguistic Approaches to the Psychology of Language: Research in the Tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin (pp. 427–439). New York: Psychology Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sandra, D., & Rice, S. (1995). Network analyses of prepositional meaning: Mirroring whose mind—the linguist’s or the language user’s? Cognitive Linguistics, 6(1), 89–130. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1995.6.1.89 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sapir, E. (1929). The Status of Linguistics as a Science. Language, 5(4), 207–214. https://doi.org/10.2307/409588 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sarrazin, J.-C., Giraudo, M.-D., & Vercher, J.-L. (2008). Coupling kinematics of memory and kinematics of movement: The conditions for a psychological relativity. Human Movement Science, 27(3), 532–550. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2007.09.003 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sarrazin, J.-C., Tonnelier, A., & Alexandre, F. (2005). A model of contextual effect on reproduced extents in recall tasks: The issue of the imputed motion hypothesis. Biological Cybernetics, 92(5), 303–315. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-005-0553-3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sato, M. (2010). Message in the “body”: Effects of simulation in sentence production (Ph.D. Dissertation). University of Hawaii, Mānoa. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sato, M., & Bergen, B. K. (2013). The case of the missing pronouns: Does mentally simulated perspective play a functional role in the comprehension of person? Cognition, 127(3), 361–374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.02.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sattig, T. (2006). The Language and Reality of Time. Oxford: Clarendon Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Saygin, A. P., McCullough, S., Alac, M., & Emmorey, K. (2010). Modulation of BOLD Response in Motion-sensitive Lateral Temporal Cortex by Real and Fictive Motion Sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(11), 2480–2490. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21388 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Schmidtke, H., Tschander, L., Eschenbach, C., & Habel, C. (2003). Change of Orientation. In E. van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing Direction in Language and Space (pp. 166–190). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Schönefeld, D. (2013). Go mad – come true – run dry: Metaphorical motion, semantic preference(s) and deixis. Yearbook of the German Cognitive Linguistics Association, 1(1), 215–235. https://doi.org/10.1515/gcla-2013-0012 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Semin, G. R., & Smith, E. R. (Eds.). (2008). Embodied Grounding: Social, Cognitive, Affective, and Neuroscientific Approaches. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Semino, E., & Culpeper, J. (Eds.). (2002). Cognitive Stylistics: Language and Cognition in Text Analysis. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Seuren, P. A. M. (2013). From Whorf to Montague: Explorations in the Theory of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sharifian, F. (2017). Cultural Linguistics: Cultural Conceptualisations and Language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sheets-Johnstone, M. (2011). The Primacy of Movement, Expanded 2nd Ed. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Shibatani, M. (2002). Introduction: Some basic issues in the grammar of causation. In M. Shibatani (Ed.), The Grammar of Causation and Interpersonal Manipulation (pp. 1–22). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinclair, J. (1991). Corpus, Concordance, Collocation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinclair, J. (1996). Preliminary recommendations on Corpus Typology. (EAGLES Guidelines 1996 No. EAG--TCWG--CTYP/P). Istituto di Linguistica Computazionale, Pisa: Expert Advisory Group on Language Engineering Standards. Retrieved on 15.07.2001 from http://www.ilc.cnr.it/EAGLES/corpustyp/corpustyp.html | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinclair, J. (2003a). Corpus processing. In P. van Sterkenburg (Ed.), A Practical Guide to Lexicography (pp. 179–193). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinclair, J. (2003b). Reading Concordances: An Introduction. London: Pearson Longman. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinclair, J. (2004). Trust the Text: Language, Corpus and Discourse. London: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinclair, J. (2005). Corpus and Text — Basic Principles. In W. Martin (Ed.), Developing Linguistic Corpora: A Guide to Good Practice (pp. 1–16). Oxford: Oxbow Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Singh, N., & Mishra, R. K. (2010). Simulating Motion in Figurative Language Comprehension. The Open Neuroimaging Journal, 4, 46–52. https://doi.org/10.2174/1874440001004010046 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slack, J., & van der Zee, E. (2003). The Representation of Direction in Language and Space. In E. van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing Direction in Language and Space (pp. 1–17). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (1987). Thinking for Speaking. In Proceedings of the Thirteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 435–445). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (1996a). From “though and language” to “thinking for speaking”. In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking Linguistic Relativity (pp. 70–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (1996b). Two Ways to Travel: Verbs of Motion in English and Spanish. In M. Shibatani & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Grammatical Constructions: Their Form and Meaning (pp. 195–219). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (1997). Mind, Code, and Text. In J. L. Bybee, J. Haiman, & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Essays on Language Function and Language Type: Dedicated to T. Givón (pp. 437–467). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (2003). Language and thought online: Cognitive consequences of linguistic relativity. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought (pp. 157–192). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (2004). The Many Ways to Search for a Frog: Linguistic Typology and the Expression of Motion Events. In S. Strömqvist & L. T. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating Events in Narrative, Vol. 2: Typological and Contextual Perspectives (pp. 219–257). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (2005). Relating Narrative Events in Translation. In D. D. Ravid & H. B.-Z. Shyldkrot (Eds.), Perspectives on Language and Language Development (pp. 115–129). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (2006). What makes manner of motion salient? Explorations in linguistic typology, discourse, and cognition. In M. Hickmann & S. Robert (Eds.), Space in Languages: Linguistic Systems and Cognitive Categories (pp. 59–81). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (2009). Relations between Paths of Motion and Paths of Vision. In V. C. Mueller Gathercole (Ed.), Routes to Language: Studies in Honor of Melissa Bowerman (pp. 197–222). New York: Psychology Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I., & Hoiting, N. (1994). Reference to Movement in Spoken and Signed Languages. In Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: General Session Dedicated to the Contributions of Charles J. Fillmore (pp. 487–505). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I., Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I., Kopecka, A., & Majid, A. (2014). Manners of human gait: A crosslinguistic event-naming study. Cognitive Linguistics, 25(4). https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2014-0061 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slowik, E. (2017). Descartes’ Physics. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2017). Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved on 11.04.2018 from https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2017/entries/descartes-physics/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Smart, J. J. C. (Ed.). (1964). Problems of Space and Time. New York: Macmillan. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Smilek, D., Callejas, A., Dixon, M. J., & Merikle, P. M. (2007). Ovals of time: Time-space associations in synaesthesia. Consciousness and Cognition, 16(2), 507–519. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2006.06.013 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Snell-Hornby, M. (1983). Verb-descriptivity in German and English: A contrastive study in semantic fields. Heidelberg: Carl Winter Universitätsverlag. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Solstad, T., & Bott, O. (2017). Causality and Causal Reasoning in Natural Language. In M. Waldmann (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Causal Reasoning (pp. 619–644). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sorabji, R. (1988). Matter, Space and Motion: Theories in Antiquity and Their Sequel. London: Duckworth. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Spivey, M. J., & Geng, J. J. (2001). Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent objects. Psychological Research, 65(4), 235–241. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004260100059 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Srinivasan, M., & Carey, S. (2010). The long and the short of it: On the nature and origin of functional overlap between representations of space and time. Cognition, 116(2), 217–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stanfield, R. A., & Zwaan, R. A. (2001). The Effect of Implied Orientation Derived from Verbal Context on Picture Recognition. Psychological Science, 12(2), 153–156. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00326 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stefanowitsch, A., & Rohde, A. (2004). The goal bias in the encoding of motion events. In G. Radden & K.-U. Panther (Eds.), Studies in Linguistic Motivation (pp. 249–267). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Steinman, R. M., Pizlo, Z., & Pizlo, F. J. (2000). Phi is not beta, and why Wertheimer’s discovery launched the Gestalt revolution. Vision Research, 40(17), 2257–2264. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00086-9 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stewart, D. (2010). Semantic Prosody: A Critical Evaluation. New York: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stocker, K. (2014). The Theory of Cognitive Spacetime. Metaphor and Symbol, 29(2), 71–93. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2014.889991 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stocker, K. (2015). Toward an Embodied Cognitive Semantics. Cognitive Semantics, 1(2), 178–212. https://doi.org/10.1163/23526416-00102002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stockwell, P. (2016). Cognitive stylistics. In R. H. Jones (Ed.), The Routledge Handbook of Language and Creativity (pp. 218–230). London: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stosic, D., & Sarda, L. (2009). The many ways to be located: The expression of fictive motion in French and Serbian. In M. B. Vukanović & L. G. Grmuša (Eds.), Space and Time in Language and Literature (pp. 39–60). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stosic, D., Fagard, B., Sarda, L., & Colin, C. (2015). Does the road go up the mountain? Fictive motion between linguistic conventions and cognitive motivations. Cognitive Processing, 16(S1), 221–225. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-015-0723-8 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stubbs, M. (1993). British Traditions in Text Analysis — From Firth to Sinclair. In M. Baker, G. Francis, & E. Tognini-Bonelli (Eds.), Text and Technology: In Honour of John Sinclair (pp. 1–33). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/z.64.02stu | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stubbs, M. (1995). Collocations and semantic profiles: On the cause of the trouble with quantitative studies. Functions of Language, 2(1), 23–55. https://doi.org/10.1075/fol.2.1.03stu | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sullivan, K. (2017). Conceptual Metaphor. In B. Dancygier (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 385–406). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Svorou, S. (1994). The Grammar of Space. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sweetser, E. (1988). Grammaticalization and Semantic Bleaching. In S. Axmaker, A. Jaisser, & H. Singmaster (Eds.), Proceedings of the 14th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 389–405). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. https://doi.org/10.3765/bls.v14i0.1774 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sweetser, E. (1997). Role and individual interpretations of change predicates. In J. Nuyts & E. Pederson (Eds.), Language and Conceptualization (pp. 116–136). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Szwedek, A. (2009). Conceptualization of space and time. In P. Łobacz, W. Zabrocki, & P. Nowak (Eds.), Language, Science and Culture: Essays in Honor of Professor Jerzy Bańczerowski on the Occasion of His 70th Birthday (pp. 317–333). Poznań: Wydawnictwo Naukowe UAM. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Szwedek, A. (2011). The ultimate source domain. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 9(2), 341–366. https://doi.org/10.1075/rcl.9.2.01szw | pl_PL |
dc.references | Szwedek, A. (2014). The nature of domains and the relationships between them in metaphorization. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 12(2), 342–374. https://doi.org/10.1075/rcl.12.2.04szw | pl_PL |
dc.references | Szwedek, A. (2018). Angels and devils lost and regained: A revision of the Great Chain of Being. Kwartalnik Neofilologiczny, LXV(1), 3–20. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Takahashi, K. (2001). Access Path Expressions in Thai. In A. J. Cienki, B. J. Luka, & M. B. Smith (Eds.), Conceptual and Discourse Factors in Linguistic Structure (pp. 237–252). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1975a). Figure and Ground in Complex Sentences. In Proceedings of the First Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 419–430). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1975b). Semantics and syntax of motion. In J. P. Kimball (Ed.), Syntax and Semantics, Vol. 4 (pp. 181–238). New York: Academic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1978). The relation of grammar to cognition: A synopis. In D. Waltz (Ed.), Proceedings of the 1978 Workshop on Theoretical Issues in Natural Language Processing (pp. 14–24). Stroudsburg, PA: Association for Computational Linguistics. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1983). How language structures space. In H. L. Pick & L. P. Acredolo (Eds.), Spatial Orientation: Theory, Research, and Application (pp. 225–282). New York: Plenum Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1985). Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language Typology and Syntactic Description, Vol. 3: Grammatical Categories and the Lexicon (pp. 57–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1988). Force Dynamics in Language and Cognition. Cognitive Science, 12(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1201_2 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1988/2007c). The relation of grammar to cognition [First published in 1988]. In V. Evans, B. K. Bergen, & J. Zinken (Eds.), The Cognitive Linguistics Reader (pp. 481–544). London: Equinox. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1991). Path to Realization: A Typology of Event Conflation. In Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: General Session and Parasession on The Grammar of Event Structure (pp. 480–519). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1996). Fictive Motion in Language and “Ception.” In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and Space (pp. 211–276). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2000a). Toward a Cognitive Semantics, Vol. I: Concept Structuring Systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2000b). Toward a Cognitive Semantics, Vol. II: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2005a). Leonard Talmy. A windowing to conceptual structure and language, Part 1: Lexicalisation and typology [Interview by Iraide Ibarretxe-Antuñano]. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 3(1), 325–347. https://doi.org/10.1075/arcl.3.17iba | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2005b). The fundamental system of spatial schemas in language. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 199–234). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2007a). Foreword [Comparing introspection with other methodologies]. In M. Gonzalez-Marquez, I. Mittelberg, S. Coulson, & M. J. Spivey (Eds.), Methods in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. xi–xxi). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2007b). Lexical typologies. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language Typology and Syntactic Description (2nd ed., pp. 66–168). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511618437.002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2009). Main Verb Properties and Equipollent Framing. In J. Guo, E. Lieven, N. Budwig, S. Ervin-Tripp, K. Nakamura, & Ş. Özçalişkan (Eds.), Crosslinguistic Approaches to the Psychology of Language: Research in the Tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin (pp. 389–402). New York: Psychology Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2011). Cognitive Semantics: An overview. In C. Maienborn, K. von Heusinger, & P. Portner (Eds.), Semantics: An International Handbook of Natural Language Meaning (Vol. 1, pp. 622–642). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268(5217), 1632–1634. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7777863 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1996). Perspective in Spatial Descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(3), 371–391. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0021 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Taylor, J. R. (1996). On running and jogging. Cognitive Linguistics, 7(1), 21–34. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1996.7.1.21 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Taylor, L. J., & Zwaan, R. A. (2009). Action in cognition: The case of language. Language and Cognition, 1(01), 45–58. https://doi.org/10.1515/LANGCOG.2009.003 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tesnière, L. (1959/2015). Elements of structural syntax [First published in 1959 as Éléments de syntaxe structurale]. (T. J. Osborne & S. Kahane, Trans.). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Teubert, W. (2005). My version of corpus linguistics. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 10(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1075/ijcl.10.1.01teu | pl_PL |
dc.references | Thelen, E. (1995). Motor Development: A New Synthesis. American Psychologist, 50(2), 79–95. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.50.2.79 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Thelen, E. (2000). Motor development as foundation and future of developmental psychology. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24(4), 385–397. https://doi.org/10.1080/016502500750037937 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Therriault, D. J., & Rinck, M. (2007). Multidimensional situation models. In F. Schmalhofer & C. A. Perfetti (Eds.), Higher Level Language Processes in the Brain: Inference and Comprehension Processes (pp. 311–327). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tognini-Bonelli, E. (2001). Corpus Linguistics at Work. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tomczak, E., & Ewert, A. (2015). Real and Fictive Motion Processing in Polish L2 Users of English and Monolinguals: Evidence for Different Conceptual Representations. The Modern Language Journal, 99(Supplement), 49–65. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12178 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tommasi, L., & Laeng, B. (2012). Psychology of spatial cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(6), 565–580. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1198 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2006). Flexible Conceptual Projection of Time Onto Spatial Frames of Reference. Cognitive Science, 30(4), 745–757. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_67 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Traugott, E. C. (1989). On the Rise of Epistemic Meanings in English: An Example of Subjectification in Semantic Change. Language, 65(1), 31–55. https://doi.org/10.2307/414841 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Traugott, E. C. (2006). Semantic Change: Bleaching, Strengthening, Narrowing, Extension. In K. Brown (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics, Second Edition (pp. 124–131). Oxford: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B0-08-044854-2/01105-6 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Traugott, E. C. (2010). (Inter)subjectivity and (inter)subjectification: A reassessment. In K. Davidse, L. Vandelanotte, & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), Subjectification, Intersubjectification and Grammaticalization (pp. 29–74). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Trojszczak, M. (2016). Metaphorical reification of mind in linguistic representations. A cognitive study based on Polish and English corpora (Ph.D. Dissertation). University of Łódź, Łódź. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tsujimura, N. (2001). A constructional approach to stativity in Japanese. Studies in Language, 25(3), 601–629. https://doi.org/10.1075/sl.25.3.07tsu | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tummers, J., Heylen, K., & Geeraerts, D. (2005). Usage-based approaches in Cognitive Linguistics: A technical state of the art. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 1(2), 225–261. https://doi.org/10.1515/cllt.2005.1.2.225 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Turner, M. (2007). Conceptual Integration. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 377–393). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1131. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.185.4157.1124 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2001). Spatial schemas in depictions. In M. Gattis (Ed.), Spatial Schemas and Abstract Thought (pp. 79–112). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2002). What do Sketches Say about Thinking? In D. Randall, L. James, & S. Tom (Eds.), Architectures for Intelligent Theory-Based Agents: Papers from 2002 AAAI Spring Symposium (pp. 140–147). Menlo Park, CA: AAAI Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2003). Structures Of Mental Spaces: How People Think About Space. Environment & Behavior, 35(1), 66–80. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916502238865 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2005). Visuospatial Reasoning. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning (pp. 209–240). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2009). Spatial Cognition: Embodied and Situated. In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition (pp. 201–216). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2011). Visualizing Thought. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3(3), 499–535. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01113.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B., & Hard, B. M. (2009). Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking. Cognition, 110(1), 124–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.008 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ugaglia, M. (2016). The Actuality of the Movable (by the Mover): A Relational Interpretation of Aristotle’s Definition of Motion. Rhizomata, 4(2), 225–256. https://doi.org/10.1515/rhiz-2016-0012 | pl_PL |
dc.references | van der Zee, E., & Slack, J. (Eds.). (2003). Representing Direction in Language and Space. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | van der Zee, E., Nikanne, U., & Sassenberg, U. (2010). Grain levels in English path curvature descriptions and accompanying iconic gestures. Journal of Spatial Information Science, (1), 95–113. https://doi.org/10.5311/JOSIS.2010.1.7 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Vandeloise, C. (1994). Methodology and analyses of the preposition in. Cognitive Linguistics, 5(2), 157–184. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1994.5.2.157 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Vendler, Z. (1957). Verbs and Times. Philosophical Review, 66(2), 143–160. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Vendler, Z. (1967). Linguistics in Philosophy. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Verkuyl, H. J. (1993). A Theory of Aspectuality: The Interaction Between Temporal and Atemporal Structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Vulchanova, M., Martinez, L., & Vulchanov, V. (2012). Distinctions in the linguistic encoding of motion: Evidence from a free naming task. In M. Vulchanova & E. van der Zee (Eds.), Motion Encoding in Language and Space (pp. 11–43). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Vulchanova, M., & van der Zee, E. (Eds.). (2013). Motion encoding in language and space. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wagner, M. (2006). The Geometries of Visual Space. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2005). Typologia korpusów oraz warsztat informatyczny lingwistyki korpusowej [Corpus typology and the computational workshop of corpus linguistics]. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Podstawy językoznawstwa korpusowego (pp. 27–41). Łódź: Łódź University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2014a). Atemporality of Coextension Paths. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk & K. Kosecki (Eds.), Time and Temporality in Language and Human Experience (pp. 103–119). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2014b). Complementarity of Space and Time in Distance Representations: A Cognitive Corpus-based Study, 2nd Ed. Łódź: Łódź University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2015a). Application of conceptual conditions for translation of fictive motion. In P. Pietrzak & M. Deckert (Eds.), Constructing Translation Competence (pp. 75–89). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2015b). Instrumentality of fictive motion in coextension paths. Kwartalnik Neofilologiczny, 62(1), 87–103. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2016a). Construal in approximations of caused-motion patterns. In D. Gonigroszek & A. Stanecka (Eds.), Philological Studies: Interdisciplinary and Multidimensional Approaches (pp. 67–83). Piotrków Trybunalski: Naukowe Wydawnictwo Piotrkowskie. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2016b). How to sneeze off papers from the desk in Polish translation: Re-conceptualization and approximation at work. In Ł. Bogucki, B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, & M. Thelen (Eds.), Translation and Meaning. New Series, Vol. 2, Pt. 1 (pp. 85–105). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2017a). Fictive motion expressions that encode the source and goal of motion in the British National Corpus (PELCRA Research Report No. 01/JTW/2017). Łódź: University of Łódź. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.31640.44806 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2017b). Unbounded path verbs in fictive motion in the British National Corpus (PELCRA Research Report No. 02/JTW/2017). Łódź: University of Łódź. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.21574.11843 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2017c). Relations of routing (cross and pass) in coextension paths in the British National Corpus (PELCRA Research Report No. 03/JTW/2017). Łódź: University of Łódź. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.33318.16966 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2017d). Chase and accompany verbs in fictive motion expressions in the British National Corpus (PELCRA Research Report No. 04/JTW/2017). Łódź: University of Łódź. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.29962.72649 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2017e). Deictic verbs of motion in coextension paths in the British National Corpus (PELCRA Research Report No. 05/JTW/2017). Łódź: University of Łódź. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.36673.61284 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2018a). Verbs of rolling in coextension path expressions in the British National Corpus (PELCRA Research Report No. 01/JTW/2018). Łódź: University of Łódź. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.30277.29925 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2018b). Verbs of walking in fictive motion in the British National Corpus (PELCRA Research Report No. 02/JTW/2018). Łódź: University of Łódź. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.26921.85607 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2018c). Verbs of running in fictive motion expressions in the British National Corpus (PELCRA Research Report No. 03/JTW/2018). Łódź: University of Łódź. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.33632.74242 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2018d). Fictive motion expressions including verbs of unsteady movement in the British National Corpus (PELCRA Research Report No. 04/JTW/2018). Łódź: University of Łódź. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.17694.38729 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2018e). Instrumental motion verbs in coextension path expressions in the British National Corpus (PELCRA Research Report No. 05/JTW/2018) [Data first published in 2013 as COST TIMELY Research Report No. 06/2013]. Łódź: University of Łódź. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.32793.88169 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T., & Pęzik, P. (2007). Web access interface to the PELCRA referential corpus of Polish. In J. T. Waliński, K. Kredens, & S. Goźdź-Roszkowski (Eds.), Corpora and ICT in Language Studies (pp. 65–86). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wallentin, M., Østergaard, S., Lund, T. E., Østergaard, L., & Roepstorff, A. (2005). Concrete spatial language: See what I mean? Brain and Language, 92(3), 221–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.06.106 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483–488. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Warglien, M., Gärdenfors, P., & Westera, M. (2012). Event structure, conceptual spaces and the semantics of verbs. Theoretical Linguistics, 38(3–4). https://doi.org/10.1515/tl-2012-0010 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wertheimer, M. (1912). Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen von Bewegung [Experimental Studies in the Perception of Movement]. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie, 61, 161–265. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wheeler, J. A., & Ford, K. W. (2010). Geons, Black Holes, and Quantum Foam: A Life in Physics. New York: W.W. Norton & Company. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Whorf, B. L. (1939/1956a). The Relation of Habitual Thought and Behavior to Language [Written in 1939]. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 134–159). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Whorf, B. L. (1940/1956b). Science and Linguistics [Written in 1940]. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 207–219). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wierzbicka, A. (1996). Semantics: Primes and Universals. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wierzbicka, A. (2006). English: Meaning and Culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wilkins, D. P., & Hill, D. (1995). When ˝go˝ means ˝come˝: Questioning the basicness of basic motion verbs. Cognitive Linguistics, 6(2–3), 209–260. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1995.6.2-3.209 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wilks, Y. (1975). Preference semantics. In E. L. Keenan (Ed.), Formal semantics of Natural Language (pp. 329–348). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511897696.022 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Williams, J. M. (1976). Synaesthetic Adjectives: A Possible Law of Semantic Change. Language, 52(2), 461–478. https://doi.org/10.2307/412571 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Williams, R. F. (2004). Making meaning from a clock: Material artifacts and conceptual blending in time-telling instruction. (Ph.D. Dissertation). University of California, San Diego. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 625–636. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196322 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wilson, R. A., & Foglia, L. (2015). Embodied Cognition. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2015). Retrieved on 21.09.2017 from https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/embodied-cognition/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Winter, B., & Bergen, B. K. (2012). Language comprehenders represent object distance both visually and auditorily. Language and Cognition, 4(01), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1515/langcog-2012-0001 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Winterboer, A., Tenbrink, T., & Moratz, R. (2013). Spatial directionals for robot navigation. In M. Vulchanova & E. van der Zee (Eds.), Motion Encoding in Language and Space (pp. 84–101). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wittgenstein, L. (1953/2009). Philosophical Investigations, Rev. 4th Ed. [Philosophische Untersuchungen, first published, posthumously, in 1953]. (G. E. M. Anscombe, P. M. S. Hacker, & J. Schulte, Trans.). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Woelert, P. (2011). Human cognition, space, and the sedimentation of meaning. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 10(1), 113–137. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-010-9153-3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wolff, P., & Holmes, K. J. (2011). Linguistic relativity. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(3), 253–265. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.104 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wychorska, E. (2014). Abstract vs concrete: Contrastive analysis of the conceptualization of stillness and motion in Polish and English. In K. Rudnicka-Szozda & A. Szwedek (Eds.), Cognitive Linguistics in the Making (pp. 349–360). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wynne, M. (2008). Searching and concordancing. In A. Lüdeling & M. Kytö (Eds.), Corpus Linguistics: An International Handbook, Vol. 1 (pp. 708–737). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Yang, J. (2014). The role of the right hemisphere in metaphor comprehension: A meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 35(1), 107–122. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22160 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Yaxley, R. H., & Zwaan, R. A. (2007). Simulating visibility during language comprehension. Cognition, 105(1), 229–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.003 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Yeh, W., & Barsalou, L. W. (2006). The Situated Nature of Concepts. The American Journal of Psychology, 119(3), 349. https://doi.org/10.2307/20445349 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zacks, J. M., & Tversky, B. (2001). Event structure in perception and conception. Psychological Bulletin, 127(1), 3–21. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zipf, G. K. (1949). Human Behavior and the Principle of Least Effort. New York: Addison-Wesley. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zlatev, J. (2007). Spatial Semantics. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 318–350). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zlatev, J. (2015). Cognitive Semiotics. In P. P. Trifonas (Ed.), International Handbook of Semiotics (pp. 1043–1067). Dordrecht: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9404-6_47 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zlatev, J., Blomberg, J., & David, C. (2010). Translocation, language and the categorization of experience. In V. Evans & P. A. Chilton (Eds.), Language, Cognition and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions (pp. 389–418). London: Equinox. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zlatev, J., Blomberg, J., & Magnusson, U. (2012). Metaphor and subjective experience: A study of motion-emotion metaphors in English, Swedish, Bulgarian, and Thai. In A. Foolen, U. M. Lüdtke, T. P. Racine, & J. Zlatev (Eds.), Moving Ourselves, Moving Others: Motion and emotion in intersubjectivity, consciousness and language. (pp. 423–450). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zlatev, J., & Yangklang, P. (2004). A Third Way to Travel: The Place of Thai in Motion-Event Typology. In S. Strömqvist & L. T. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating Events in Narrative, Vol. 2: Typological and Contextual Perspectives (pp. 159–190). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A. (2004). The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 44 (pp. 35–62). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Time in Language, Situation Models, and Mental Simulations. In P. Indefrey & M. Gullberg (Eds.), Time to Speak: Cognitive and Neural Prerequisites for Time in Language (pp. 13–26). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A., & Pecher, D. (2012). Revisiting Mental Simulation in Language Comprehension: Six Replication Attempts. PLoS ONE, 7(12), e51382. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0051382 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123(2), 162–185. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.123.2.162 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A., Madden, C. J., Yaxley, R. H., & Aveyard, M. E. (2004). Moving words: Dynamic representations in language comprehension. Cognitive Science, 28(4), 611–619. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsci.2004.03.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A., Stanfield, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2002). Language Comprehenders Mentally Represent the Shapes of Objects. Psychological Science, 13(2), 168–171. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00430 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A., & Taylor, L. J. (2006). Seeing, Acting, Understanding: Motor Resonance in Language Comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwarts, J. (2003). Vectors across Spatial Domains: From Place to Size, Orientation, Shape, and Parts. In E. van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing Direction in Language and Space (pp. 39–68). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwarts, J. (2004). Competition between word meanings: The polysemy of (a)round. In C. Meier & M. Weisgerber (Eds.), Proceedings of sub8-Sinn und Bedeutung (pp. 349–360). Konstanz: University of Konstanz Fachbereich Sprachwissenschaft. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwarts, J. (2008). Aspects of a typology of direction. In S. D. Rothstein (Ed.), Theoretical and Crosslinguistic Approaches to the Semantics of Aspect (pp. 79–105). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwarts, J. (2017). Spatial semantics: Modeling the meaning of prepositions. Language and Linguistics Compass, 11(5), e12241. https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12241 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwarts, J., & Gärdenfors, P. (2016). Locative and Directional Prepositions in Conceptual Spaces: The Role of Polar Convexity. Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 25(1), 109–138. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10849-015-9224-5 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chaucer, G. (1387/1997). The Man of Law's Tale [Written c. 1387]. In S. Kökbugur (Ed.), The Canterbury Tales and Other Works by Geoffrey Chaucer. Librarius. Available at: www.librarius.com | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gervais, R. (Producer & Director), & Robinson, M. (Director). (2009). The Invention of Lying [Motion picture]. United States: Warner Bros. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Shakespeare, W. (1603/1966). Hamlet, Prince of Denmark [First printed in 1603]. In W. J. Craig (Ed.), The Complete Works of William Shakespeare. London: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Shakespeare, W. (1623/1966). The Tragedy of Macbeth [First printed in 1623]. In W. J. Craig (Ed.), The Complete Works of William Shakespeare. London: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tolkien, J. R. R. (1954/2012). The Fellowship of the Ring. New York: Del Rey. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tolkien, J. R. R. (1954/2012). La comunidad del Anillo. (Luis Domènech, Spanish Trans.). Mexico: Editorial Planeta Mexicana. | pl_PL |
dc.references | BNC. (2001). The British National Corpus [World Edition] Oxford: Oxford University Computing Services. Available from OUCS at: http://www.natcorp.ox.ac.uk | pl_PL |
dc.references | Google Books Ngram Viewer. (2013). Mountain View, CA: Google LLC. Available at: https://books.google.com/ngrams/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Oxford English Dictionary, 2nd Edition, Version 4.0. (2009). [CD-ROM]. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | SlopeQ for the BNC. (2017). A part-of-speech-sensitive search engine with support for proximity queries for the British National Corpus data. A general description of the SlopeQ query syntax can be found at: http://pelcra.pl/docs/doku.php?id=slopeq_for_bnc | pl_PL |
dc.references | VerbNet 3.2. (2013). A class-based verb lexicon with mappings to other lexical resources. The unified verb index accessible from: http://verbs.colorado.edu/verb-index/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | WordNet Online Search 3.1. (2010). A lexical database for English. Princeton: Princeton University. Available at: http://wordnetweb.princeton.edu/perl/webwn | pl_PL |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.18778/8142-382-3 | |