dc.contributor.author | Waliński, Jacek Tadeusz | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2017-11-09T17:53:28Z | |
dc.date.available | 2017-11-09T17:53:28Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2014 | |
dc.identifier.citation | Waliński J. T., Complementarity of Space and Time in Distance Representations. Second edition, Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego, Łódź 2014, doi: 10.18778/7969-441-9 | pl_PL |
dc.identifier.isbn | 978-83-7969-441-9 | |
dc.identifier.uri | http://hdl.handle.net/11089/23202 | |
dc.description | Second edition. This book presents a cognitive linguistic study of distance representations carried out using the British National Corpus and the National Corpus of Polish. Corpusbased examination of linguistic expressions of spatial expanse in the semantic context of motion events suggests that as language users we are at certain liberty to choose either spatial or temporal conceptualization of distance according to what suits our subjective profiling needs relevant to a particular situation.
From the perspective of research presented in this book, the entanglement of time and space in cognition is a rabbit hole phenomenon that runs deep beyond the ties that bind space to time in motion, on the one hand, and the socio-cultural sedimentation of meaning through commonly used phraseology, on the other. It appears to hinge on a higher-order ontological and epistemological distinction between objects and events, which may influence our spatial-temporal reasoning.
Additionally, this book demonstrates a cognitive schema of temporaI horizon emergent from the frequency of expressions denoting temporaI distance found in spontaneous informal conversations. Taken together the research presented in this volume indicates that, at least in certain contexts, space and time can act in a complementary manner in cognition. | pl_PL |
dc.language.iso | en | pl_PL |
dc.publisher | Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego | pl_PL |
dc.rights | Uznanie autorstwa-Użycie niekomercyjne-Bez utworów zależnych 3.0 Polska | * |
dc.rights.uri | http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/pl/ | * |
dc.subject | space | pl_PL |
dc.subject | time | pl_PL |
dc.subject | linguistic perspective | pl_PL |
dc.subject | language | pl_PL |
dc.subject | cognition | pl_PL |
dc.subject | distance | pl_PL |
dc.title | Complementarity of Space and Time in Distance Representations. | pl_PL |
dc.type | Book | pl_PL |
dc.rights.holder | © Copyright by University of Łódź, Łódź 2014 | pl_PL |
dc.page.number | 298 | pl_PL |
dc.contributor.authorAffiliation | University of Łódź, Faculty of Philology Institute of English Studies, 90-236 Łódź, Pomorska 171/173 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ahn, H.-K., Liu, M. W., & Soman, D. (2009). Memory markers: How consumers recall the duration of experiences. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19(3), 508–516. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2009.05.002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Alexander, H. G. (Ed.). (1956). The Leibniz-Clarke Correspondence: Together with Extracts from Newton’s Principia and Optiks. Manchester: Manchester University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Alexander, S. (1966). Space, time, and Deity, Vols. 1-2) [First published in 1920]. New York: Dover. Retrieved from http://www.giffordlectures.org/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Allwood, J. (2002). Language and Time. Publications of the Department of General Linguistics of the University of Tartu, Finland, 3, 1–8. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Alverson, H. (1994). Semantics and Experience: Universal Metaphors for Time in English, Mandarin, Hindi and Sesotho. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aristotle. (335BC/1995a). Poetics [written c. 335BC]. In J. Barnes (Ed.), The Complete Works of Aristotle (Vol. 2). Princeton, N.J: Princeton University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aristotle. (350BC/1995b). Physics [written c. 350BC]. In J. Barnes (Ed.), The Complete Works of Aristotle (Vol. 1). Princeton, N.J: Princeton University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aristotle. (350BC/1995c). On the Soul [written c. 350BC]. In J. Barnes (Ed.), The Complete Works of Aristotle (Vol. 1). Princeton, N.J: Princeton University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aschersleben, G., & Müsseler, J. (2010). On the perceived interdependence of space and time: evidence for spatial priming in the temporal kappa effect. In R. Nijhawan & B. Khurana (Eds.), Space and Time in Perception and Action (pp. 181–196). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aske, J. (1989). Path Predicates in English and Spanish: A Closer Look. In Proceedings of the Fifteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 1–14). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Aston, G., & Burnard, L. (1998). The BNC Handbook: Exploring the British National Corpus with SARA. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Augustine, S. (398AD/1978). Confessions: Saint Augustine [First written in Latin in 398AD as Confessiones]. (R. S. Pine-Coffin, Trans.). Harmondsworth: Penguin Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Baayen, R. H. (2008). Analyzing Linguistic Data: A Practical Introduction to Statistics Using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bailey, N., & Areni, C. S. (2006). When a few minutes sound like a lifetime: Does atmospheric music expand or contract perceived time? Journal of Retailing, 82(3), 189–202. doi:10.1016/j.jretai.2006.05.003 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barnes, J. (1982). The Presocratic Philosophers. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 577–660. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18(5-6), 513–562. doi:10.1080/01690960344000026 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded Cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 617– 645. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1521), 1281– 1289. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0319 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bączkowska, A. (2011). Space, Time & Language: A Cognitive Analysis of English Prepositions. Bydgoszcz: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Kazimierza Wielkiego. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Beavers, J., Levin, B., & Wei Tham, S. (2010). The typology of motion expressions revisited. Journal of Linguistics, 46(02), 331–377. doi:10.1017/ S0022226709990272 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Behnke, E. A. (2011). Husserl’s Phenomenology of Embodiment. In (J. Fieser & B. Dowden, Eds.) Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved from http://www.iep.utm.edu/husspemb/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ben-Zvi, P. (2005). Kant on Space. Philosophy Now, 49, 14–16. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bender, A., Beller, S., & Bennardo, G. (2010). Temporal Frames of Reference: Conceptual Analysis and Empirical Evidence from German, English, Mandarin Chinese and Tongan. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 10(3), 283–307. doi:10.1163/156853710X531195 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bender, A., Bennardo, G., & Beller, S. (2005). Spatial Frames of Reference for Temporal Relations: A Conceptual Analysis in English, German, and Tongan. In B. G. Bara, L. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 220–225). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bender, A., Rothe-Wulf, A., Hüther, L., & Beller, S. (2012). Moving forward in space and time: how strong is the conceptual link between spatial and temporal frames of reference? Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 486. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00486 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bennardo, G. (2004). Linguistic untranslatability vs. conceptual nesting of frames of reference. In K. Forbus, D. Gentner, & T. Regier (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 102–107). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bennett, D. C. (1975). Spatial and Temporal Uses of English Prepositions: An Essay in Stratificational Semantics. London: Longman. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergen, B. K. (2012). Louder Than Words: The New Science of How the Mind Makes Meaning. New York: Basic Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergen, B. K., & Chan Lau, T. T. (2012). Writing direction affects how people map space onto time. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 109. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00109 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergen, B. K., Lindsay, S., Matlock, T., & Narayanan, S. (2007). Spatial and Linguistic Aspects of Visual Imagery in Sentence Comprehension. Cognitive Science, 31(5), 733–764. doi:10.1080/03640210701530748 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergson, H. (1889/2001). Time and Free Will, an Essay on the Immediate Data of Consciousness [First published in 1889 as Essai sur les données immédiates de la conscience]. (F. L. Pogson, Trans.). New York: Dover. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergson, H. (1922/1999). Duration and Simultaneity [First published in 1922 as Durée et simultanéité]. (L. Jacobson, Trans.). Manchester: Clinamen Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergson, H. (1922/1999). Duration and Simultaneity [First published in 1922 as Durée et simultanéité]. (L. Jacobson, Trans.). Manchester: Clinamen Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bergson, H. (2002). Henri Bergson: Key Writings. (K. Ansell-Pearson & J. Mullarkey, Eds.). New York: Continuum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bernard, E., & Griffin, J. (2009). Understanding Lucene’s query syntax. In Hibernate Search in Action (pp. 202–214). Greenwich, CT: Manning. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Biber, D. (1993). Representativeness in Corpus Design. Literary and Linguistic Computing, 8(4), 243–257. doi:10.1093/llc/8.4.243 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Biber, D., Conrad, S., & Reppen, R. (1998). Corpus Linguistics: Investigating Language Structure and Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G. N., Conrad, S., & Finegan, E. (1999). Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow: Pearson Longman. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bielak, J., & Pawlak, M. (2013). Applying Cognitive Grammar in the Foreign Language Classroom: Teaching English Tense and Aspect. Berlin: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Black, M. (1962). Models and Metaphors: Studies in Language and Philosophy. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Black, M. (1979/1993). More about metaphor [First published in 1979]. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and Thought, 2nd Ed. (pp. 19–41). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Block, R. A. (1990). Models of psychological time. In R. A. Block (Ed.), Cognitive Models of Psychological Time (pp. 1–35). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Block, R. A., Buggie, S. E., & Matsui, F. (1996). Beliefs About Time: Cross-Cultural Comparisons. The Journal of Psychology, 130(1), 5–22. doi:10.1080/ 00223980.1996.9914984 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Block, R. A., Hancock, P. A., & Zakay, D. (2010). How cognitive load affects duration judgments: A meta-analytic review. Acta Psychologica, 134(3), 330–343. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.03.006 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (2006). Prospective Remembering Involves Time Estimation and Memory Processes. In J. Glicksohn & M. S. Myslobodsky (Eds.), Timing the Future: The Case for a Time-Based Prospective Memory (pp. 25–49). Hackensack, NJ: World Scientific. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Block, R. A., & Zakay, D. (2008). Timing and Remembering the Past, the Present, and the Future. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of Time (pp. 367–394). Bingley: Emerald. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Blomberg, J., & Zlatev, J. (2013). Actual and non-actual motion: why experientialist semantics needs phenomenology (and vice versa). Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, ePub ahead of print. doi:10.1007/s11097-013-9299-x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bloom, P., & Keil, F. C. (2001). Thinking Through Language. Mind and Language, 16(4), 351–367. doi:10.1111/1468-0017.00175 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bonato, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2012). When time is space: Evidence for a mental time line. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(10), 2257–2273. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.007 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 1–28. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00073-6 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does Language Shape Thought?: Mandarin and English Speakers’ Conceptions of Time. Cognitive Psychology, 43(1), 1–22. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0748 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boroditsky, L. (2011a). How Language Shapes Thought. Scientific American, 304(2), 62– 65. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0211-62 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boroditsky, L. (2011b). How Languages Construct Time. In S. Dehaene & E. M. Brannon (Eds.), Space, Time and Number in the Brain (pp. 333–341). London: Academic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boroditsky, L., Fuhrman, O., & McCormick, K. (2011). Do English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently? Cognition, 118(1), 123–129. doi:10.1016/ j.cognition.2010.09.010 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boroditsky, L., & Gaby, A. (2010). Remembrances of Times East: Absolute Spatial Representations of Time in an Australian Aboriginal Community. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1635–1639. doi:10.1177/0956797610386621 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M. (2002). The Roles of Body and Mind in Abstract Thought. Psychological Science, 13(2), 185–189. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00434 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bowdle, B. F., & Gentner, D. (2005). The Career of Metaphor. Psychological Review, 112(1), 193–216. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.193 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bower, G. H., & Morrow, D. G. (1990). Mental models in narrative comprehension. Science, 247(4938), 44–48. doi:10.1126/science.2403694 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Boyd, J. N., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1997). Constructing Time After Death: The Transcendental- Future Time Perspective. Time & Society, 6(1), 35–54. doi:10.1177/ 0961463X97006001002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brandt, L., & Brandt, P. A. (2005). Making sense of a blend: A cognitive-semiotic approach to metaphor. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 3(1), 216–249. doi:10.1075/arcl.3.12bra | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brandt, P. A. (2005). Mental spaces and cognitive semantics: A critical comment. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(10), 1578–1594. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2004.10.019 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bréal, M. (1897). Semantics: Studies in the Science of Meaning [French title: Essai de sémantique]. (H. Cust, Trans.). New York: Henry Holt. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brewer, B., & Pears, J. (1993). Frames of Reference. In N. Eilan, R. A. McCarthy, & B. Brewer (Eds.), Spatial Representation: Problems in Philosophy and Psychology (pp. 25–30). Oxford: Blackwell. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Broccias, C., & Hollmann, W. B. (2007). Do we need summary and sequential scanning in (Cognitive) grammar? Cognitive Linguistics, 18(4), 487–522. doi:10.1515/COG.2007.026 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brown, P. (2006). A sketch of the grammar of space in Tzeltal. In S. C. Levinson & D. P. Wilkins (Eds.), Grammars of Space: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity (pp. 230– 272). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brown, P. (2012). Time and Space in Tzeltal: Is the Future Uphill? Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 212. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00212 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brugman, C. M. (1988). The Story of Over: Polysemy, Semantics, and the Structure of the Lexicon [Originally presented as the author’s MA thesis, University of California, Berkeley, 1981]. New York: Garland Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Brugman, C. M., & Lakoff, G. (1988). Cognitive Topology and Lexical Networks. In S. L. Small, G. W. Cottrell, & M. K. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Lexical Ambiguity Resolution: Perspectives from Psycholinguistics, Neuropsychology, and Artificial Intelligence (pp. 477–508). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bueti, D., & Walsh, V. (2009). The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1831–1840. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0028 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Buhusi, C. V., & Meck, W. H. (2005). What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(10), 755–765. doi:10.1038/nrn1764 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Burnett, P. (1978). Time Cognition and Urban Travel Behavior. Geografiska Annaler, Series B, Human Geography, 60(2), 107. doi:10.2307/490634 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Butler, D. L., & Overshiner, C. (1983). The role of mental computations in judgments of area and volume. Perception & Psychophysics, 34(6), 593–598. doi:10.3758/ BF03205916 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bybee, J. L. (2006). From Usage to Grammar: The Mind’s Response to Repetition. Language, 82(4), 711–733. doi:10.1353/lan.2006.0186 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bybee, J. L. (2010). Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bybee, J. L., & Fleischman, S. (Eds.). (1995). Modality in Grammar and Discourse. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cacciari, C., Bolognini, N., Senna, I., Pellicciari, M. C., Miniussi, C., & Papagno, C. (2011). Literal, fictive and metaphorical motion sentences preserve the motion component of the verb: A TMS study. Brain and Language, 119(3), 149–157. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2011.05.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cadierno, T. (2008). Learning to talk about motion in a foreign language. In P. Robinson & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition (pp. 239–275). New York: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cadierno, T. (2010). Motion in Danish as a Second Language: Does the Learner’s L1 Make a Difference? In Z.-H. Han & T. Cadierno (Eds.), Linguistic Relativity in SLA: Thinking for Speaking (pp. 1–33). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cadwallader, M. (1979). Problems in Cognitive Distance: Implications for Cognitive Mapping. Environment and Behavior, 11(4), 559–576. doi:10.1177/ 0013916579114007 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cann, R. (1993). Formal Semantics: An Introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cantlon, J. F., & Brannon, E. M. (2006). Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans. Psychological Science, 17(5), 401–406. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01719.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cantlon, J. F., Platt, M. L., & Brannon, E. M. (2009). Beyond the number domain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 83–91. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.007 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Carenini, G., Murray, G., & Ng, R. (2011). Methods for Mining and Summarizing Text Conversations. San Rafael, CA: Morgan & Claypool. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Carlson, L. A. (2010). Encoding Space in Spatial Language. In K. S. Mix, L. B. Smith, & M. Gasser (Eds.), The Spatial Foundations of Language and Cognition (pp. 157–183). Oxford: Oxford University Press | pl_PL |
dc.references | Carlson, L. A., & Covey, E. S. (2005). How far is near? Inferring distance from spatial descriptions. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20(5), 617–631. doi:10.1080/ 01690960400023501 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Casasanto, D. (2008). Who’s Afraid of the Big Bad Whorf? Crosslinguistic Differences in Temporal Language and Thought. Language Learning, 58, 63–79. doi:10.1111/j.1467- 9922.2008.00462.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Casasanto, D. (2009). When is a linguistic metaphor a conceptual metaphor? In V. Evans & S. Pourcel (Eds.), New Directions in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 127–145). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106(2), 579–593. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Casasanto, D., Fotakopoulou, O., & Boroditsky, L. (2010). Space and Time in the Child’s Mind: Evidence for a Cross-Dimensional Asymmetry. Cognitive Science, 34(3), 387– 405. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01094.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Casati, R., & Varzi, A. (2010). Events. In (E. N. Zalta, Ed.) The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2010/entries/events/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Castells, M. (2000). The Rise of the Network Society, 2nd Ed. Oxford: Blackwell. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chen, J.-Y. (2007). Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104(2), 427–436. doi:10.1016/ j.cognition.2006.09.012 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1962). A transformational approach to syntax. In Proceedings of the Third Texas Conference on Problems of Linguistic Analysis in English on May 9-12, 1958 (pp. 124–158). Austin: University of Texas | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1970). Remarks on nominalization. In R. A. Jacobs & P. S. Rosenbaum (Eds.), Readings in English Transformational Grammar (pp. 184–221). Waltham, MA: Ginn. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on Language. New York: Pantheon Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin, and Use. New York: Praeger. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Chomsky, N. (1997). Noise: Noam Chomsky interviewed by Fred Branfman. HotWired, (February, 1997). Retrieved from http://www.chomsky.info/interviews/199702--.htm | pl_PL |
dc.references | Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Clayton, K., & Habibi, A. (1991). Contribution of temporal contiguity to the spatial priming effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(2), 263–271. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.17.2.263 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cohen, J. (1967). Relativity of psychological time. In Psychological time in health and disease (pp. 40–57). Springfield, IL: C.C. Thomas. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cohen, J., Hansel, C. E. M., & Sylvester, J. D. (1953). A New Phenomenon in Time Judgment. Nature, 172(4385), 901–901. doi:10.1038/172901a0 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cohen, J., Hansel, C. E. M., & Sylvester, J. D. (1954). Interdependence of Temporal and Auditory Judgments. Nature, 174(4431), 642–644. doi:10.1038/174642a0 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cohen, J., Hansel, C. E. M., & Sylvester, J. D. (1955). Interdependence in judgments of space, time and movement. Acta Psychologica, 11, 360–372. doi:10.1016/S0001- 6918(55)80098-4 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Collyer, C. E. (1977). Discrimination of spatial and temporal intervals defined by three light flashes: Effects of spacing on temporal judgments and of timing on spatial judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 21(4), 357–364. doi:10.3758/ BF03199487 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Comrie, B. (1976). Aspect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Comrie, B. (1985). Tense. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Corballis, M. C. (2013a). Mental time travel: a case for evolutionary continuity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(1), 5–6. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.009 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Corballis, M. C. (2013b). The wandering rat: response to Suddendorf. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(4), 152. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.012 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cottle, T. J. (1967). The Circles Test: An Investigation of Perceptions of Temporal Relatedness and Dominance. Journal of Projective Techniques and Personality Assessment, 31(5), 58–71. doi:10.1080/0091651X.1967.10120417 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cottle, T. J. (1968). The location of experience: A manifest time orientation. Acta Psychologica, 28, 129–149. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(68)90009-7 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cottle, T. J., & Pleck, J. H. (1969). Linear Estimations of Temporal Extension: The Effect of Age, Sex, and Social Class. Journal of Projective Techniques and Personality Assessment, 33(1), 81–93. doi:10.1080/0091651X.1969.10120563 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Coulson, S. (2001). Semantic leaps: Frame-shifting and conceptual blending in meaning construction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Coulson, S., & Matlock, T. (2001). Metaphor and the Space Structuring Model. Metaphor and Symbol, 16(3-4), 295–316. doi:10.1080/10926488.2001.9678899 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Coulson, S., & Oakley, T. (2000). Blending basics. Cognitive Linguistics, 11(3-4), 175–196. doi:10.1515/cogl.2001.014 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Coulson, S., & Oakley, T. (2005). Blending and coded meaning: Literal and figurative meaning in cognitive semantics. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(10), 1510–1536. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2004.09.010 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Crawley, S. E., & Pring, L. (2000). When did Mrs Thatcher resign? The effects of ageing on the dating of public events. Memory, 8(2), 111–121. doi:10.1080/ 096582100387650 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Croft, W. (1998). The Structure of Events and the Structure of Language. In M. Tomasello (Ed.), The New Psychology of Language: Cognitive and Functional Approaches to Language Structure, Vol. 1 (pp. 67–92). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Croft, W. (2006). On explaining metonymy: Comment on Peirsman and Geeraerts, “Metonymy as a prototypical category.” Cognitive Linguistics, 17(3), 317–326. doi:10.1515/COG.2006.008 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Croft, W. (2009). Aspectual and Causal Structure in Event Representations. In V. C. Mueller-Gathercole (Ed.), Routes to Language: Studies in Honor of Melissa Bowerman (pp. 139–166). New York: Psychology Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Croft, W., & Cruse, D. A. (2004). Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Crowder, R. G., & Neath, I. (1994). The Influence of Pitch on Time Perception in Short Melodies. Music Perception, 12(4), 379–386. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cruse, D. A. (1986). Lexical Semantics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Curiel, J. M., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Mental organization of maps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(1), 202–214. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.24.1.202 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Cytowic, R. E., & Eagleman, D. M. (2009). Wednesday Is Indigo Blue: Discovering the Brain of Synesthesia. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | D’Alessio, M., Guarino, A., De Pascalis, V., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2003). Testing Zimbardo’s Stanford Time Perspective Inventory (STPI) - Short Form: An Italian Study. Time & Society, 12(2), 333–347. doi:10.1177/0961463X030122010 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dainton, B. (2010a). Temporal Consciousness. In (E. N. Zalta, Ed.) The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2010/entries/consciousness-temporal/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dainton, B. (2010b). Supplement to Temporal Consciousness: Some Relevant Empirical Findings (Psychology, Psychophysics, Neuroscience). In (E. N. Zalta, Ed.) The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/consciousness-temporal/empirical-findings.html | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dancygier, B., & Sweetser, E. (2005). Mental Spaces in Grammar: Conditional Constructions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Davidson, D. (1967/2001). The Logical Form of Action Sentences [First published in 1967]. In Essays on Actions and Events, 2nd Ed. (pp. 105–122). Oxford: Clarendon Press | pl_PL |
dc.references | Davidson, D. (1978). What Metaphors Mean. Critical Inquiry, 5(1), 31–47. doi:10.1086/447971 | pl_PL |
dc.references | De Hevia, M.-D., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). Number-Space Mapping in Human Infants. Psychological Science, 21(5), 653–660. doi:10.1177/0956797610366091 | pl_PL |
dc.references | De Smedt, J., & De Cruz, H. (2011). The role of material culture in human time representation: Calendrical systems as extensions of mental time travel. Adaptive Behavior, 19(1), 63–76. doi:10.1177/1059712310396382 | pl_PL |
dc.references | De Sousa, H. (2012). Generational Differences in the Orientation of Time in Cantonese Speakers as a Function of Changes in the Direction of Chinese Writing. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 255. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00255 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The Mental Representation of Parity and Number Magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371–396. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dehaene, S., & Brannon, E. M. (Eds.). (2011). Space, Time and Number in the Brain: Searching for the Foundations of Mathematical Thought. London: Academic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Deignan, A. (2005). Metaphor and Corpus Linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Deignan, A. (2008). Corpus Linguistics and Metaphor. In R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought (pp. 280–294). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Denis, M. (1997). The description of routes: A cognitive approach to the production of spatial discourse. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 16(4), 409–458. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Deutsch, K. W., & Isard, W. (1961). A note on a generalized concept of effective distance. Behavioral Science, 6(4), 308–311. doi:10.1002/bs.3830060406 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dewell, R. B. (1994). Over again: Image-schema transformations in semantic analysis. Cognitive Linguistics, 5(4), 351–380. doi:10.1515/cogl.1994.5.4.351 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Dewell, R. B. (2005). Dynamic patterns of CONTAINMENT. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 369–393). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | DiSalle, R. (2006). Understanding Space-time: The Philosophical Development of Physics from Newton to Einstein. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | DiSalle, R. (2009). Space and Time: Inertial Frames. In (E. N. Zalta, Ed.) The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2009/entries/spacetime-iframes/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Doob, L. W. (1971). Patterning of Time. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Draaisma, D. (2004). Why Life Speeds up as You Get Older: How Memory Shapes Our Past. (A. Pomerans & E. Pomerans, Trans.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Drake, L., Duncan, E., Sutherland, F., Abernethy, C., & Henry, C. (2008). Time Perspective and Correlates of Wellbeing. Time & Society, 17(1), 47–61. doi:10.1177/0961463X07086304 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Droege, P. (2009). Now or never: How consciousness represents time. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(1), 78–90. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2008.10.006 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Droit-Volet, S., Bigand, E., Ramos, D., & Bueno, J. L. O. (2010). Time flies with music whatever its emotional valence. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 226–232. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.003 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Droit-Volet, S., & Meck, W. H. (2007). How emotions colour our perception of time. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(12), 504–513. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.008 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Einstein, A. (1916/1952a). The Foundation of the General Theory of Relativity. [First published in 1916 as Die Grundlage der allgemeinen Relativitätstheorie, Annalen der Physik, 354, 769-822]. In W. Perrett & G. B. Jeffery (Trans.), The Principle of Relativity: A Collection of Original Papers on the Special and General Theory of Relativity (pp. 109–164). New York: Dover. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Einstein, A. (1905/1952b). On the Electrodynamics of Moving Bodies [First published in 1905 as Zur Elektrodynamik bewegter Körper. Annalen der Physik, 322, 891–921]. In W. Perrett & G. B. Jeffery (Trans.), The Principle of Relativity: A Collection of Original Papers on the Special and General Theory of Relativity (pp. 35–65). New York: Dover. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Emmorey, K. (2001). Space on hand: The exploitation of signing space to illustrate abstract thought. In M. Gattis (Ed.), Spatial Schemas and Abstract Thought (pp. 147–174). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Engberg-Pedersen, E. (1999). Space and Time. In J. Allwood & P. Gärdenfors (Eds.), Cognitive Semantics: Meaning and Cognition (pp. 131–152). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Engelbert, M., & Carruthers, P. (2010). Introspection. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(2), 245–253. doi:10.1002/wcs.4 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Eriksen, T. H. (2001). Tyranny of the Moment: Fast and Slow Time in the Information Age. London: Pluto Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2003). The Structure of Time: Language, meaning and temporal cognition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2005). The meaning of time: polysemy, the lexicon and conceptual structure. Journal of Linguistics, 41(1), 33–75. doi:10.1017/S0022226704003056 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2006). Lexical concepts, cognitive models and meaning-construction. Cognitive Linguistics, 17(4), 491–534. doi:10.1515/COG.2006.016 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2009). How Words Mean: Lexical Concepts, Cognitive Models, and Meaning Construction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2010). From the spatial to the non-spatial: The “state” lexical concepts of in, on and at. In V. Evans & P. A. Chilton (Eds.), Language, Cognition and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions (pp. 215–248). London: Equinox. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2012). Cognitive linguistics. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(2), 129–141. doi:10.1002/wcs.1163 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2013a). Temporal frames of reference. Cognitive Linguistics, 24(3), 393–435. doi:10.1515/cog-2013-0016 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V. (2013b). Language and Time: A Cognitive Linguistics Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V., & Chilton, P. A. (Eds.). (2010). Language, Cognition and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions. London: Equinox. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Evans, V., & Green, M. (2006). Cognitive Linguistics: An Introduction. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Everett, D. L. (2005). Cultural Constraints on Grammar and Cognition in Pirahã: Another Look at the Design Features of Human Language. Current Anthropology, 46(4), 621–646. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Eysenck, H. J. (1959). Personality and the estimation of time. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 9(3), 405–406. doi:10.2466/pms.1959.9.3.405 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fabiszak, M. (2008). Corpus frequency as a guide to metaphor labelling. In Z. Wąsik & T. Komendziński (Eds.), Metaphor and Cognition (pp. 149–162). Frankfurt am Mein: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G. (1994). Mental Spaces: Aspects of Meaning Construction in Natural Language [First published in 1985 by MIT Press]. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G. (1997). Mappings in Thought and Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G. (2007). Mental Spaces. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 351–376). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G. (2009). Generalized integration networks. In V. Evans & S. Pourcel (Eds.), New Directions in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 147–160). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G., & Lakoff, G. (2010). On Metaphor and Blending [Announced as forthcoming in Cognitive Semiotics]. Retrieved from http://www.cogsci.ucsd.edu /~coulson/spaces/GG-final-1.pdf | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G., & Sweetser, E. (Eds.). (1996). Spaces, Worlds, and Grammar. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (1994). Conceptual Projection and Middle Spaces (Report No. 9401). San Diego, CA: Department of Cognitive Science, University of California. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (1998). Conceptual Integration Networks. Cognitive Science, 22(2), 133–187. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2202_1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2000). Compression and global insight. Cognitive Linguistics, 11(3-4), 283–304. doi:10.1515/cogl.2001.017 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2002). The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind’s Hidden Complexities. New York: Basic Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2008). Rethinking metaphor. In R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought (pp. 53–66). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Feldman, J. A. (2006). From Molecule to Metaphor: A Neural Theory of Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fellbaum, C. (Ed.). (1998). WordNet: An Electronic Lexical Database. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fellbaum, C. (2006). WordNet(s). In (K. Brown, Ed.) Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics, Second Edition. Oxford: Elsevier. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fias, W., van Dijck, J.-P., & Gevers, W. (2011). How is Number Associated with Space? The Role of Working Memory. In S. Dehaene & E. M. Brannon (Eds.), Space, Time and Number in the Brain (pp. 133–148). London: Academic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Filipović, L. (2007). Talking About Motion: A Crosslinguistic Investigation of Lexicalization Patterns. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fillmore, C. J. (1971/1997). Lectures on Deixis [First published in 1971 as Santa Cruz Lectures on Deixis]. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fillmore, C. J. (1976). Frame Semantics and the Nature of Language. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 280(1), 20–32. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1976. tb25467.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fillmore, C. J. (1982/2007). Frame semantics [First published in 1982]. In V. Evans, B. K. Bergen, & J. Zinken (Eds.), The Cognitive Linguistics Reader (pp. 238–262). London: Equinox. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fingerman, K. L., & Perlmutter, M. (1995). Future Time Perspective and Life Events across Adulthood. The Journal of General Psychology, 122(1), 95–111. doi:10.1080/00221309.1995.9921225 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fischer, K. (2010). Quantitative Methods in Cognitive Semantics. In D. Glynn & K. Fischer (Eds.), Quantitative Methods in Cognitive Semantics: Corpus-Driven Approaches (pp. 43–59). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fisiak, J., Lipińska-Grzegorek, M., & Zabrocki, T. (1987). An introductory English-Polish contrastive grammar, 2nd Ed. Warszawa: Państwowe Wydawnictwo Naukowe. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fitch, W. T., Hauser, M. D., & Chomsky, N. (2005). The evolution of the language faculty: Clarifications and implications. Cognition, 97(2), 179–210. doi:10.1016/ j.cognition.2005.02.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Flaherty, M. G. (1993). Conceptualizing Variation in the Experience of Time. Sociological Inquiry, 63(4), 394–405. doi:10.1111/j.1475-682X.1993.tb00320.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Flaherty, M. G. (1999). A Watched Pot: How We Experience Time. New York: New York University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fodor, J. A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind: An Essay on Faculty Psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fodor, J. A. (2008). LOT 2: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford: Clarendon Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Forceville, C. (2004). Review of “The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind’s Hidden Complexities.” Metaphor and Symbol, 19(1), 83–89. doi:10.1207/ S15327868MS1901_5 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fraisse, P. (1963). The Psychology of Time. New York: Harper & Row. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fraisse, P. (1978). Time and rhythm perception. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of Perception, Vol. 8: Perceptual Coding (pp. 203–254). New York: Academic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fraisse, P. (1984). Perception and Estimation of Time. Annual Review of Psychology, 35(1), 1–36. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.35.020184.000245 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fraisse, P. (1994). Rhythm. In (R. J. Corsini, Ed.) Encyclopedia of Psychology. New York: John Wiley. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Frank, L. K. (1939). Time perspectives. Journal of Social Philosophy, 4, 293–312. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Frankish, K., & Ramsey, W. (Eds.). (2012). The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Freundschuh, S. M., & Egenhofer, M. J. (1997). Human conceptions of spaces: Implications for GIS. Transactions in GIS, 2(4), 361–375. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9671.1997.tb00063.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Friedman, A., & Montello, D. R. (2006). Global-scale location and distance estimates: Common representations and strategies in absolute and relative judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32(2), 333–346. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.333 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Friedman, W. J. (2003). The Development of a Differentiated Sense of the Past and the Future. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior (Vol. 31, pp. 229–269). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Friedman, W. J., & Janssen, S. M. J. (2010). Aging and the speed of time. Acta Psychologica, 134(2), 130–141. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.01.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fuhrman, O., & Boroditsky, L. (2010). Cross-Cultural Differences in Mental Representations of Time: Evidence From an Implicit Nonlinguistic Task. Cognitive Science, 34(8), 1430–1451. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01105.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fuhrman, O., McCormick, K., Chen, E., Jiang, H., Shu, D., Mao, S., & Boroditsky, L. (2011). How Linguistic and Cultural Forces Shape Conceptions of Time: English and Mandarin Time in 3D. Cognitive Science, 35(7), 1305–1328. doi:10.1111/j.1551- 6709.2011.01193.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Fujita, K., Henderson, M. D., Eng, J., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2006). Spatial Distance and Mental Construal of Social Events. Psychological Science, 17(4), 278–282. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01698.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gaby, A. (2012). The Thaayorre think of time like they talk of space. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 300. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00300 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Galton, A. (2011). Time flies but space does not: Limits to the spatialisation of time. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(3), 695–703. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2010.07.002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Garside, R., Leech, G. N., & McEnery, T. (1997). Corpus Annotation: Linguistic Information from Computer Text Corpora. London: Longman. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Garside, R., Leech, G. N., & Sampson, G. (1987). The Computational Analysis of English: A Corpus-Based Approach. London: Longman. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Geeraerts, D. (2006). Methodology in Cognitive Linguistics. In G. Kristiansen, M. Achard, R. Dirven, & F. J. Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez (Eds.), Cognitive Linguistics: Current Applications and Future Perspectives (pp. 21–49). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Geeraerts, D. (2009). Prisms and blends: Digging one’s grave from two perspectives. In W. Oleksy & P. Stalmaszczyk (Eds.), Cognitive Approaches to Language and Linguistic Data: Studies of Honor of Barbara Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (pp. 87–104). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Geeraerts, D. (2010). The doctor and the semantician. In D. Glynn & K. Fischer (Eds.), Quantitative Methods in Cognitive Semantics: Corpus-Driven Approaches (pp. 63– 78). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gell, A. (1992). The Anthropology of Time: Cultural Constructions of Temporal Maps and Images. Oxford: Berg. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gennari, S. P., Sloman, S. A., Malt, B. C., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). Motion events in language and cognition. Cognition, 83(1), 49–79. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00166-4 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gentner, D. (2001). Spatial metaphors in temporal reasoning. In M. Gattis (Ed.), Spatial Schemas and Abstract Thought (pp. 203–222). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gentner, D., Bowdle, B. F., Wolff, P., & Boronat, C. (2001). Metaphor is like analogy. In D. Gentner, K. J. Holyoak, & B. N. Kokinov (Eds.), The Analogical Mind: Perspectives from Cognitive Science (pp. 199–253). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (Eds.). (2003). Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gentner, D., Imai, M., & Boroditsky, L. (2002). As time goes by: Evidence for two systems in processing space—time metaphors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17(5), 537– 565. doi:10.1080/01690960143000317 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gentner, D., & Jeziorski, M. (1993). The shift from metaphor to analogy in Western science. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and Thought (pp. 447–480). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2003). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized. Cognition, 87(3), B87–B95. doi:10.1016/S0010- 0277(02)00234-2 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar Timing in Memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423(Timing and Time Perception), 52–77. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23417.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibbs, R. W. (1992). What do idioms really mean? Journal of Memory and Language, 31(4), 485–506. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(92)90025-S | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibbs, R. W. (2000). Making good psychology out of blending theory. Cognitive Linguistics, 11(3-4), 347–358. doi:10.1515/cogl.2001.020 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibbs, R. W. (2005). Embodiment and Cognitive Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibbs, R. W. (2007). Idioms and Formulaic Language. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 697–725). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibbs, R. W. (Ed.). (2008). The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibbs, R. W., & Matlock, T. (2008). Metaphor, imagination, and simulation. In R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought (pp. 161–176). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gibson, J. J. (1975). Events are Perceivable But Time Is Not. In J. T. Fraser & N. Lawrence (Eds.), The Study of Time II: Proceedings of the Second Conference of the International Society for the Study of Time (pp. 295–301). Berlin: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gilquin, G., & Gries, S. T. (2009). Corpora and experimental methods: A state-of-the-art review. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 5(1), 1–26. doi:10.1515/ CLLT.2009.001 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glebkin, V. V. (2013). A Critical View on Conceptual Blending Theory. In M. Knauff, N. Sebanz, M. Pauen, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2404–2409). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gleick, J. (2000). Faster: The Acceleration of Just About Everything. New York: Vintage Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gleitman, L., & Papafragou, A. (2012). New Perspectives on Language and Thought. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning (pp. 543–568). Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20(1), 1–55. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 558–565. doi:10.3758/BF03196313 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glucksberg, S. (2001). Understanding Figurative Language: From Metaphors to Idioms. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glucksberg, S., & McGlone, M. S. (1999). When love is not a journey: What metaphors mean. Journal of Pragmatics, 31(12), 1541–1558. doi:10.1016/S0378-2166(99)00003-X | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glynn, D. (in press). Polysemy and synonymy: Cognitive theory and corpus method. In D. Glynn & J. Robinson (Eds.), Polysemy and Synonymy: Corpus Methods and Applications in Cognitive Semantics (pp. 205–238). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glynn, D. (2010). Corpus-driven Cognitive Semantics: Introduction to the field. In D. Glynn & K. Fischer (Eds.), Quantitative Methods in Cognitive Semantics: Corpus- Driven Approaches (pp. 1–41). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glynn, D., & Fischer, K. (Eds.). (2010). Quantitative Methods in Cognitive Semantics: Corpus-Driven Approaches. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Glynn, D., & Robinson, J. (Eds.). (in press). Polysemy and Synonymy: Corpus Methods and Applications in Cognitive Semantics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goddard, C., & Wierzbicka, A. (2009). Contrastive semantics of physical activity verbs: “Cutting” and “chopping” in English, Polish, and Japanese. Language Sciences, 31(1), 60–96. doi:10.1016/j.langsci.2007.10.002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goffman, E. (1974). Frame Analysis: An Essay on the Organization of Experience. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goldberg, A. E. (2003). Constructions: a new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(5), 219–224. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00080-9 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goldberg, A. E., & Suttle, L. (2010). Construction grammar. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(4), 468–477. doi:10.1002/wcs.22 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Goldreich, D. (2007). A Bayesian Perceptual Model Replicates the Cutaneous Rabbit and Other Tactile Spatiotemporal Illusions. PLoS ONE, 2(3), e333. doi:10.1371/ journal.pone.0000333 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Górska, E. (2009). On the Diversity of Linguistic Evidence for Conceptual Metaphor. Studia Anglica Posnaniensia, 45(2), 81–106. doi:10.2478/v10121-009-0017-2 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grady, J. E. (1997). Foundations of Meaning: Primary Metaphors and Primary Scenes (Ph.D. Dissertation). University of California, Berkeley. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grady, J. E. (2005). Primary metaphors as inputs to conceptual integration. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(10), 1595–1614. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2004.03.012 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grady, J. E. (2007). Metaphor. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 188–213). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grady, J. E., Coulson, S., & Oakley, T. (1999). Blending and metaphor. In R. W. Gibbs & G. Steen (Eds.), Metaphor in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 101–124). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grant, E. (1981). Much Ado About Nothing: Theories of Space and Vacuum from the Middle Ages to the Scientific Revolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T. (2003). Towards a corpus-based identification of prototypical instances of constructions. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 1(1), 1–27. doi:10.1075/arcl.1.02gri | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T. (2006). Corpus-based methods and cognitive semantics: The many meanings of to run. In S. T. Gries & A. Stefanowitsch (Eds.), Corpora in Cognitive Linguistics: Corpus-Based Approaches to Syntax and Lexis (pp. 57–99). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T. (2009). What is Corpus Linguistics? Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(5), 1225–1241. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2009.00149.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T. (2011). Studying syntactic priming in corpora: Implications of different levels of granularity. In D. Schönefeld (Ed.), Converging Evidence: Methodological and Theoretical Issues for Linguistic Research (pp. 143–165). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T. (2012). Corpus linguistics, theoretical linguistics, and cognitive/ psycholinguistics: Towards more and more fruitful exchanges. In J. Mukherjee & M. Huber (Eds.), Corpus Linguistics and Variation in English: Theory and Description (pp. 41–63). Amsterdam: Rodopi. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T. (2013). Statistics for Linguistics with R: A Practical Introduction, Second Revised Edition. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T., & Divjak, D. (2009). Behavioral profiles: A corpus-based approach to cognitive semantic analysis. In V. Evans & S. Pourcel (Eds.), New Directions in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 57–75). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T., & Divjak, D. (2010). Quantitative approaches in usage-based Cognitive Semantics: Myths, erroneous assumptions, and a proposal. In D. Glynn & K. Fischer (Eds.), Quantitative Methods in Cognitive Semantics: Corpus-Driven Approaches (pp. 333–353). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gries, S. T., & Stefanowitsch, A. (Eds.). (2006). Corpora in Cognitive Linguistics: Corpus- Based Approaches to Syntax and Lexis. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grondelaers, S., Geeraerts, D., & Speelman, D. (2007). A case for a Cognitive corpus linguistics. In M. Gonzalez-Marquez, I. Mittelberg, S. Coulson, & M. J. Spivey (Eds.), Methods in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 149–169). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grondin, S. (Ed.). (2008). Psychology of Time. Bingley: Emerald. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grondin, S. (2010). Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(3), 561–582. doi:10.3758/APP.72.3.561 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Grothe, B. (2003). Sensory systems: New roles for synaptic inhibition in sound localization. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(7), 540–550. doi:10.1038/nrn1136 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gruber, R. P., Wagner, L. F., & Block, R. A. (2004). Relationships between subjective time and information processed (reduction of uncertainty). In P. Harris & M. Crawford (Eds.), Time and Uncertainty (pp. 188–203). Boston: Brill. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Gumperz, J. J., & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.). (1996). Rethinking Linguistic Relativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hampe, B. (Ed.). (2005). From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hancock, P. A., & Block, R. A. (2012). The Psychology of Time: A View Backward and Forward. American Journal of Psychology, 125(3), 267–274. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hancock, P. A., & Rausch, R. (2010). The effects of sex, age, and interval duration on the perception of time. Acta Psychologica, 133(2), 170–179. doi:10.1016/ j.actpsy.2009.11.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hancock, P. A., & Weaver, J. L. (2005). On time distortion under stress. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 6(2), 193–211. doi:10.1080/14639220512331325747 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Harber, K. D., Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (2003). Participant Self-Selection Biases as a Function of Individual Differences in Time Perspective. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 25(3), 255–264. doi:10.1207/S15324834BASP2503_08 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Harder, P. (2003). Mental spaces: Exactly when do we need them? Cognitive Linguistics, 14(1), 91–96. doi:10.1515/cogl.2003.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Harre, R. (1994). Newton, Isaac (1642–1727). In (S. L. Macey, Ed.) Encyclopedia of Time. New York: Garland. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hasko, V. (2010). The Role of Thinking for Speaking in Adult L2 Speech: The Case of (Non)unidirectionality Encoding by American Learners of Russian. In Z.-H. Han & T. Cadierno (Eds.), Linguistic Relativity in SLA: Thinking for Speaking (pp. 34–58). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hasko, V., & Perelmutter, R. (Eds.). (2010). New Approaches to Slavic Verbs of Motion. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Haspelmath, M. (1997). From Space to Time: Temporal Adverbials in the World’s Languages. Munich: LINCOM Europa. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Haspelmath, M. (2003). The Geometry of Grammatical Meaning: Semantic Maps and Cross-Linguistic Comparison. In M. Tomasello (Ed.), The New Psychology of Language: Cognitive and Functional Approaches to Language Structure, Vol. 2 (pp. 211–242). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The Faculty of Language: What Is It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579. doi:10.1126/science.298.5598.1569 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hawking, S. (1988). A Brief History of Time: From the Big Bang to Black Holes. New York: Bantam Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Heidegger, M. (1927/2002). Being and Time [First published in 1927 as Sein und Zeit]. (J. Macquarrie & E. Robinson, Trans.). New York: Harper. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Helson, H. (1930). The tau effect – an example of psychological relativity. Science, 71(1847), 536–537. doi:10.1126/science.71.1847.536 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Helson, H., & King, S. M. (1931). The tau effect: an example of psychological relativity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14(3), 202–217. doi:10.1037/h0071164 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Henderson, M. D., Fujita, K., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2006). Transcending the “Here”: The Effect of Spatial Distance on Social Judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(5), 845–856. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.845 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hendricks, J. (2001). It’s About Time. In S. H. McFadden & R. C. Atchley (Eds.), Aging and the Meaning of Time a Multidisciplinary Exploration (pp. 21–50). New York: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Henry, M. J., McAuley, J. D., & Zaleha, M. (2009). Evaluation of an imputed pitch velocity model of the auditory tau effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(6), 1399– 1413. doi:10.3758/APP.71.6.1399 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Herskovits, A. (1985). Semantics and Pragmatics of Locative Expressions. Cognitive Science, 9(3), 341–378. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0903_3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Herskovits, A. (1986). Language and Spatial Cognition: An Interdisciplinary Study of the Prepositions in English [Republished digitally in 2009]. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Heylen, K., Tummers, J., & Geeraerts, D. (2008). Methodological issues in corpus-based Cognitive Linguistics. In G. Kristiansen & R. Dirven (Eds.), Cognitive Sociolinguistics Language Variation, Cultural Models, Social Systems (pp. 91–128). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Higginbotham, J., Pianesi, F., & Varzi, A. (2000). Speaking of Events. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hill, O. W., Buggie, S. E., & Block, R. A. (2000). Culture and Beliefs About Time: Comparisons Among Black Americans, Black Africans, and White Americans. The Journal of Psychology, 134(4), 443–461. doi:10.1080/00223980009598228 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hills, D. (2012). Metaphor. In (E. N. Zalta, Ed.) The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/ archives/win2012/entries/metaphor/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Huddleston, R. D., & Pullum, G. K. (2002). The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Huddleston, R. D., & Pullum, G. K. (2005). A Student’s Introduction to English Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Huggett, N., & Hoefer, C. (2009). Absolute and Relational Theories of Space and Motion. In (E. N. Zalta, Ed.) The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2009/entries/spacetimetheories/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Husserl, E. (1917/1991). On the Phenomenology of the Consciousness of Internal Time (1893-1917) [Original title Zur Phänomenologie des inneren Zeitbewusstseins]. (J. B. Brough, Trans.). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Husserl, E. (1928/1964). The Phenomenology of Internal Time-Consciousness [First published in 1928 as Vorlesungen zur Phänomenologie des inneren Zeitbewusstseins]. (M. Heidegger, Ed.). Bloomington: Indiana University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Husserl, E. (1939/1970). The Crisis of European Sciences and Transcendental Phenomenology: An Introduction to Phenomenological Philosophy [First published in 1939]. (D. Carr, Trans.). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Hutchins, E. (2005). Material anchors for conceptual blends. Journal of Pragmatics, 37(10), 1555–1577. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2004.06.008 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Idström, A. (2010). What Inari Saami idioms reveal about the time concept of the indigenous people of Inari. Yearbook of Phraseology, 2010, 159–178. doi:10.1515/9783110222623.1.159 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ittelson, W. H. (1973). Environment perception and contemporary perceptual theory. In W. H. Ittelson (Ed.), Environment and Cognition (pp. 1–19). New York: Seminar Press | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ivry, R. B., & Schlerf, J. E. (2008). Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(7), 273–280. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Iwata, S. (1996). Motion and Extent: Two Sides of the Same Coin. Studia Linguistica, 50(3), 256–282. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9582.1996.tb00351.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1983). Semantics and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (1996). The Architecture of the Linguistic-Spatial Interface. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and Space (pp. 1–30). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (2007). Language, Consciousness, Culture: Essays on Mental Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R. (2012). Language as a source of evidence for theories of spatial representation. Perception, 41(9), 1128–1152. doi:10.1068/p7271 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R., & Aaron, D. (1991). Review of “More Than Cool Reason: A Field Guide to Poetic Metaphor.” Language, 67(2), 320–338. doi:10.2307/415109 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jackendoff, R., & Pinker, S. (2005). The nature of the language faculty and its implications for evolution of language (Reply to Fitch, Hauser, and Chomsky). Cognition, 97(2), 211–225. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.04.006 | pl_PL |
dc.references | James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology (Vols. 1-2). New York: Henry Holt. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Janda, L. A. (2004). A metaphor in search of a source domain: The categories of Slavic aspect. Cognitive Linguistics, 15(4). doi:10.1515/cogl.2004.15.4.471 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Janda, L. A. (2009). What is the role of semantic maps in cognitive linguistics? In W. Oleksy & P. Stalmaszczyk (Eds.), Cognitive Approaches to Language and Linguistic Data: Studies of Honor of Barbara Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (pp. 105–123). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Janssen, S. M. J., Naka, M., & Friedman, W. J. (2013). Why does life appear to speed up as people get older? Time & Society, 22(2), 274–290. doi:10.1177/ 0961463X13478052 | pl_PL |
dc.references | January, D., & Kako, E. (2007). Re-evaluating evidence for linguistic relativity: Reply to Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104(2), 417–426. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006. 07.008 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jaszczolt, K. M. (2009). Representing Time: An Essay on Temporality as Modality. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jaszczolt, K. M. (2011). Time as Degress of Epistemic Commitment. In P. Stalmaszczyk (Ed.), Turning Points in the Philosophy of Language and Linguistics (pp. 19–34). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jaszczolt, K. M. (2012). Cross-linguistic differences in expressing time and universal principles of utterance interpretation. In L. Filipović & K. M. Jaszczolt (Eds.), Space and Time in Languages and Cultures: Linguistic Diversity (pp. 95–121). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jensen, A. R. (1980). Bias in Mental Testing. New York: Free Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ji, L.-J., Guo, T., Zhang, Z., & Messervey, D. (2009). Looking into the past: Cultural differences in perception and representation of past information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(4), 761–769. doi:10.1037/a0014498 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jiga-Boy, G. M., Clark, A. E., & Semin, G. R. (2010). So Much to Do and So Little Time: Effort and Perceived Temporal Distance. Psychological Science, 21(12), 1811–1817. doi:10.1177/0956797610388043 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Johnson, M. (2007). The Meaning of the Body: Aesthetics of Human Understanding. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jones, B., & Huang, Y. L. (1982). Space-time dependencies in psychophysical judgment of extent and duration: Algebraic models of the tau and kappa effects. Psychological Bulletin, 91(1), 128–142. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.91.1.128 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kadosh, R. C., & Gertner, L. (2011). Synesthesia: Gluing Together Time, Number, and Space. In S. Dehaene & E. M. Brannon (Eds.), Space, Time and Number in the Brain (pp. 123–132). London: Academic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). On the study of statistical intuitions. Cognition, 11(2), 123–141. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(82)90022-1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kalisz, R. (2001). Językoznawstwo kognitywne w świetle językoznawstwa funkcjonalnego. Gdańsk: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Gdańskiego. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kalkavage, P. (1994). Descartes, Rene (1596–1650). In (S. L. Macey, Ed.) Encyclopedia of Time. New York: Garland. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kant, I. (1768/1968). Concerning the Ultimate Foundation of the Differentiation of Regions in Space [First published in 1768]. In G. B. Kerferd & D. E. Walford (Trans.), Selected Pre-Critical Writings and Correspondence with Beck (pp. 36–43). Manchester: Manchester University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kant, I. (1787/2003). Critique of Pure Reason [First published in 1787 as Kritik der reinen Vernunft]. (N. Kemp Smith, Trans.). New York: Palgrave Macmillan. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kardela, H. (1992). Gramatyka kognitywna jako globalna teoria języka. In I. Nowakowska- Kempna (Ed.), Podstawy metodologiczne semantyki współczesnej (pp. 9–22). Wrocław: Wiedza o Kulturze. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kardela, H. (2006a). Koncepcja umysłu ucieleśnionego w kognitywizmie. In W. Dziarnowska & A. Klawiter (Eds.), Mózg i jego umysły (pp. 217–243). Poznań: Wydawnictwo Zysk i S-ka. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kardela, H. (2006b). Metodologia językoznawstwa kognitywnego. In P. Stalmaszczyk (Ed.), Metodologie językoznawstwa współczesnego: Podstawy teoretyczne (pp. 196– 233). Łódź: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kardela, H. (2007). Event Structure: A Force Dynamics/Absolute Construal Account. In W. Chłopicki, A. Pawelec, & A. Pokojska (Eds.), Cognition in Language: Volume in Honour of Professor Elżbieta Tabakowska (pp. 150–167). Kraków: Tertium. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kaschak, M. P., Madden, C. J., Therriault, D. J., Yaxley, R. H., Aveyard, M. E., Blanchard, A. A., & Zwaan, R. A. (2005). Perception of motion affects language processing. Cognition, 94(3), B79–B89. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.06.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kastenbaum, R. (1961). The Dimensions of Future Time Perspective, an Experimental Analysis. The Journal of General Psychology, 65(2), 203–218. doi:10.1080/ 00221309.1961.9920473 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kastenbaum, R. (1963). Cognitive and personal futurity in later life. Journal of Individual Psychology, 19(2), 216–222. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kastenbaum, R. (1964). The structure and function of time perspective. Journal of Psychological Researches (India), 8, 1–11. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kastenbaum, R. (1994). Time perspective. In (R. J. Corsini, Ed.) Encyclopedia of Psychology. New York: John Wiley. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kay, P., & Kempton, W. (1984). What Is the Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis? American Anthropologist, 86(1), 65–79. doi:10.1525/aa.1984.86.1.02a00050 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Keefe, R. (2000). Theories of Vagueness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kelly, M. R. (2008, August). Phenomenology and Time-Consciousness. In (J. Fieser & B. Dowden, Eds.) Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved from http://www.iep.utm.edu/phe-time/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kelly, S. D. (2005). The Puzzle of Temporal Experience. In A. Brook & K. Akins (Eds.), Cognition and the Brain: The Philosophy and Neuroscience Movement (pp. 208–238). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kemmerer, D. (2005). The spatial and temporal meanings of English prepositions can be independently impaired. Neuropsychologia, 43(5), 797–806. doi:10.1016/ j.neuropsychologia.2004.06.025 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Keough, K. A., Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Who’s smoking, drinking, and using drugs? Time perspective as a predictor of substance use. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21(2), 149–164. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Keysar, B., & Bly, B. M. (1999). Swimming against the current: Do idioms reflect conceptual structure? Journal of Pragmatics, 31(12), 1559–1578. doi:10.1016/S0378- 2166(99)00004-1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Keysar, B., Shen, Y., Glucksberg, S., & Horton, W. S. (2000). Conventional Language: How Metaphorical Is It? Journal of Memory and Language, 43(4), 576–593. doi:10.1006/jmla.2000.2711 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kimmel, M. (2004). Metaphor Variation in Cultural Context: Perspectives from Anthropology. European Journal of English Studies, 8(3), 275–294. doi:10.1080/ 1382557042000277395 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Klatzky, R. L., Colledge, R. G., Loomis, J. M., Cicinelli, J. G., & Pellegrino, J. W. (1995). Performance of blind and sighted persons on spatial tasks. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 89(1), 70–82. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Koerner, E. F. K. (1992). The Sapir-Whorf Hypothesis: A Preliminary History and a Bibliographical Essay. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 2(2), 173–198. doi:10.1525/jlin.1992.2.2.173 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kopecka, A. (2010). Motion events in Polish: Lexicalization patterns and the description of Manner. In V. Hasko & R. Perelmutter (Eds.), New Approaches to Slavic Verbs of Motion (pp. 225–246). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kosecki, K. (2001). Some Remarks on Benjamin Lee Whorf’s Analysis of the Hopian Notion of Time. Acta Universitatis Lodziensis: Folia Linguistica Anglica, 3, 99–105. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kosecki, K. (2005). Metafory i metonimie czasoprzestrzeni. In J. Adamowski (Ed.), Przestrzeń w jezyku i w kulturze: problemy teoretyczne: interpretacje tekstów religijnych (pp. 91–99). Lublin: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Marii Curie- Sklodowskiej. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kosecki, K. (Ed.). (2007). Perspectives on Metonymy. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kosecki, K. (2008). Language, Time, and Biology: A Cognitive Perspective. Włocławek: Wydawnictwo PWSZ. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kotarbiński, T. (1929/1990a). Elementy teorii poznania, logiki formalnej i metodologii nauk [First published in 1929]. Wrocław: Zakład Narodowy im. Ossolińskich. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kotarbiński, T. (1990b). Philosophical Self-Portrait. In J. Woleński (Ed.), Kotarbiński: Logic, Semantics and Ontology (pp. 1–6). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kozanecka-Dymek, A. (2012). On Kinds of Temporal Logic and Some of Its Applications. Studies in Logic, Grammar and Rhetoric, 28(41), 73–81. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kövecses, Z. (1986). Metaphors of Anger, Pride and Love: A Lexical Approach to the Structure of Concepts. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kövecses, Z. (2005). Metaphor in Culture: Universality and Variation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kövecses, Z. (2006). Language, Mind, and Culture: A Practical Introduction. New York: Oxford. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kövecses, Z. (2010). Metaphor: A Practical Introduction, 2nd Ed. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kranjec, A. (2006). Extending Spatial Frames of Reference to Temporal Concepts. In R. Sun & N. Miyake (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 447–452). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kranjec, A., Cardillo, E. R., Schmidt, G. L., & Chatterjee, A. (2010). Prescribed spatial prepositions influence how we think about time. Cognition, 114(1), 111–116. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.008 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Kreitzer, A. (1997). Multiple levels of schematization: A study in the conceptualization of space. Cognitive Linguistics, 8(4), 291–326. doi:10.1515/cogl.1997.8.4.291 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Krzeszowski, T. P. (1990). The Axiological Aspect of Idealized Cognitive Models. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Meaning and Lexicography (pp. 135–165). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Krzeszowski, T. P. (1997). Angels and Devils in Hell: Elements of Axiology in Semantics. Warszawa: Energeia. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Krzeszowski, T. P. (2002). From target to source: Metaphors made real. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk & K. Turewicz (Eds.), Cognitive Linguistics Today (pp. 139–158). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Krzeszowski, T. P. (2013, September 17). Meaninglessness of “meaning”? A multidimensional view of the meta-concept. Plenary Lecture presented at the Cognitive Linguistics in the Year 2013 Conference, Warszawa, Społeczna Akademia Nauk. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal About the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G. (1993). The contemporary theory of metaphor. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and Thought, 2nd Ed. (pp. 202–251). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G. (2008). The Neural Theory of Metaphor. In R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought (pp. 17–38). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. E. (2000). Where Mathematics Comes from: How the Embodied Mind Brings Mathematics into Being. New York: Basic Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakoff, G., & Turner, M. (1989). More Than Cool Reason: A Field Guide to Poetic Metaphor. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakusta, L., & Landau, B. (2005). Starting at the end: the importance of goals in spatial language. Cognition, 96(1), 1–33. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.03.009 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lakusta, L., & Landau, B. (2012). Language and Memory for Motion Events: Origins of the Asymmetry Between Source and Goal Paths. Cognitive Science, 36(3), 517–544. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01220.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Landau, B., Dessalegn, B., & Goldberg, A. M. (2010). Language and space: Momentary interactions. In V. Evans & P. A. Chilton (Eds.), Language, Cognition and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions (pp. 51–78). London: Equinox. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Landau, B., & Jackendoff, R. (1993). “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(02), 217–265. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00029733 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (1986). Abstract Motion. In Proceedings of the Twelfth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 455–471). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (1987a). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Vol. 1: Theoretical Prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (1987b). Nouns and Verbs. Language, 63(1), 53–94. doi:10.2307/415384 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (1991). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Vol. 2: Descriptive Application. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (1992). Prepositions as grammatical(izing) elements. Leuvenese Bijdragen, 81, 287–309. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (1993). Grammatical traces of some “invisible” semantic constructs. Language Sciences, 15(4), 323–355. doi:10.1016/0388-0001(93)90008-G | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2002). Concept, Image, and Symbol: The Cognitive Basis of Grammar, 2nd Ed. [First edition published in 1991]. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2005). Dynamicity, fictivity, and scanning: The imaginative basis of logic and linguistic meaning. In D. Pecher & R. A. Zwaan (Eds.), Grounding Cognition: The Role of Perception and Action in Memory, Language, and Thinking (pp. 164–197). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2008a). Cognitive Grammar: A Basic Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2008b). Sequential and summary scanning: A reply. Cognitive Linguistics, 19(4), 533–607. doi:10.1515/COGL.2008.022 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2008c). Cognitive Grammar as a basis for language instruction. In P. Robinson & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition (pp. 66–88). New York: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langacker, R. W. (2012). Linguistic manifestations of the space-time (dis)analogy. In L. Filipović & K. M. Jaszczolt (Eds.), Space and Time in Languages and Cultures: Language, culture, and cognition (pp. 191–216). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Langston, W. (2002). Violating Orientational Metaphors Slows Reading. Discourse Processes, 34(3), 281–310. doi:10.1207/S15326950DP3403_3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Large, E. W. (2008). Resonating to Musical Rhythm: Theory and Experiment. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of Time (pp. 189–232). Bingley: Emerald. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Le Poidevin, R. (2003). Travels in Four Dimensions: The Enigmas of Space and Time. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Le Poidevin, R. (2004). A Puzzle Concerning Time Perception. Synthese, 142(1), 109–142. doi:10.1023/B:SYNT.0000047710.71824.b1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Le Poidevin, R. (2007). The Images of Time: An Essay on Temporal Representation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Le Poidevin, R. (2011). The Experience and Perception of Time. In (E. N. Zalta, Ed.) The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2011/entries/time-experience/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Le Poidevin, R., & MacBeath, M. (Eds.). (1993). The Philosophy of Time. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lee, D. (2000). Learning of spatial and temporal patterns in sequential hand movements. Cognitive Brain Research, 9(1), 35–39. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(99)00040-3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Leech, G. N. (2005). Adding Linguistic Annotation. In W. Martin (Ed.), Developing Linguistic Corpora: A Guide to Good Practice (pp. 17–29). Oxford: Oxbow Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lehrer, A. (2009). Wine & Conversation, 2nd Ed. [First published 1983 by Indiana University Press]. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Leibowitz, H. W., Guzy, L. T., Peterson, E., & Blake, P. T. (1993). Quantitative perceptual estimates: verbal versus nonverbal retrieval techniques. Perception, 22(9), 1051–1060. doi:10.1068/p221051 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levelt, W. J. M. (1982). Linearization in Describing Spatial Networks. In S. Peters & E. Saarinen (Eds.), Processes, Beliefs, and Questions (pp. 199–220). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levelt, W. J. M. (1996). Perspective Taking and Ellipsis in Spatial Descriptions. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and Space (pp. 77–108). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levelt, W. J. M. (1999). Models of word production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(6), 223–232. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01319-4 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levin, B. (1993). English Verb Classes and Alternations: A Preliminary Investigation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levinson, S. C. (1996). Language and Space. Annual Review of Anthropology, 25(1), 353– 382. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.25.1.353 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levinson, S. C. (2003). Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Levinson, S. C., & Wilkins, D. P. (Eds.). (2006). Grammars of Space: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2004). The PELCRA Project – state of the art. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Corpus Linguistics, Computers Tools, and Applications – State of the Art (pp. 9–25). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2007). Polysemy, Prototypes, and Radial Categories. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 139–169). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (Ed.). (2008a). Asymmetric Events. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2008b). Czym jest język? Dzisiejsze kontrowersje w paradygmatach generatywnych i kognitywnych. In P. Stalmaszczyk (Ed.), Metodologie językoznawstwa: współczesne tendencje i kontrowersje (pp. 9–26). Kraków: Lexis. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2008c). PALC 2007: Where are we now? In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Corpus Linguistics, Computer Tools, and Applications - State of the Art PALC 2007 (pp. 33–48). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2011). Events as They Are. In P. Stalmaszczyk (Ed.), Turning Points in the Philosophy of Language and Linguistics (pp. 35–64). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2014). Time and Time Experience in Language. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk & K. Kosecki (Eds.), Time and Temporality in Language and Human Experience (pp. 19–44). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B., & Dziwirek, K. (Eds.). (2009). Studies in Cognitive Corpus Linguistics. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewin, K. (1939). Field Theory and Experiment in Social Psychology: Concepts and Methods. American Journal of Sociology, 44(6), 868–896. doi:10.1086/218177 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewin, K. (1948). Time Perspective and Morale [First published in 1942]. In G. W. Lewin (Ed.), Resolving Social Conflicts: Selected Papers on Group Dynamics (pp. 103–124). New York: Harper. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewin, K. (1951). Field Theory in the Social Sciences: Selected Theoretical Papers. (D. Cartwright, Ed.). New York: Harper. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lewis, J. D., & Weigert, A. J. (1981). The Structures and Meanings of Social Time. Social Forces, 60(2), 432–462. doi:10.1093/sf/60.2.432 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Li, P., Abarbanell, L., Gleitman, L., & Papafragou, A. (2011). Spatial reasoning in Tenejapan Mayans. Cognition, 120(1), 33–53. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.02.012 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Liberman, N., Sagristano, M. D., & Trope, Y. (2002). The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(6), 523–534. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00535-8 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Linde, C., & Labov, W. (1975). Spatial Networks as a Site for the Study of Language and Thought. Language, 51(4), 924. doi:10.2307/412701 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lloyd, D. (2010, February 23). Neural correlates of temporality: Default mode variability and state-dependent temporal awareness. Consciousness Online. Retrieved from http://consciousnessonline.com/2010/02/19/neural-correlates-of-temporality-defaultmode- variability-and-state-dependent-temporal-awareness | pl_PL |
dc.references | Locke, J. (1689/1995). An Essay Concerning Human Understanding [First published in 1689]. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Logan, G. D., & Sadler, D. D. (1996). A Computational Analysis of the Apprehension of Spatial Relations. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and Space (pp. 493–529). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Los, B. (2005). The Rise of the To-Infinitive. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lourenco, S. F., & Longo, M. R. (2010). General Magnitude Representation in Human Infants. Psychological Science, 21(6), 873–881. doi:10.1177/0956797610370158 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lucy, J. A. (1992a). Language Diversity and Thought: A Reformulation of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lucy, J. A. (1997). Linguistic Relativity. Annual Review of Anthropology, 26(1), 291–312. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.26.1.291 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lynch, K. (1960). The Image of the City. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lyngfelt, B., & Solstad, T. (Eds.). (2006). Demoting the Agent: Passive, Middle and Other Voice Phenomena. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics (Vols. 1-2). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | MacEachren, A. M. (1980). Travel Time as the Basis of Cognitive Distance. The Professional Geographer, 32(1), 30–36. doi:10.1111/j.0033-0124.1980.00030.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mach, E. (1897). Contributions to the Analysis of the Sensations [First published in 1886 as Beiträge zur Analyse der Empfindungen]. (C. M. Williams, Trans.). Chicago: Open Court. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Magurshak, D. (1979). Heidegger and Edwards on “Sein-zum-Tode.” The Monist, 62(1), 107–118. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Maines, D. R. (1989). Culture and Temporality. Cultural Dynamics, 2(1), 107–123. doi:10.1177/092137408900200107 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Majid, A., Bowerman, M., Kita, S., Haun, D. B. M., & Levinson, S. C. (2004). Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(3), 108–114. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.003 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Malt, B. C., & Wolff, P. (Eds.). (2010). Words and the Mind: How Words Capture Human Experience. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mani, I., Pustejovsky, J., & Gaizauskas, R. (Eds.). (2005). The Language of Time: A Reader. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Marchetti, G. (2009). Studies on time: a proposal on how to get out of circularity. Cognitive Processing, 10(1), 7–40. doi:10.1007/s10339-008-0215-1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mari, A. (2006). What do the notions of instrumentality and of manner have in common? In P. Saint-Dizier (Ed.), Syntax and Semantics of Prepositions (pp. 263–287). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mari, A., & Saint-Dizier, P. (2006). A Conceptual Semantics for Prepositions denoting Instrumentality. In P. Saint-Dizier (Ed.), Syntax and Semantics of Prepositions (pp. 289–305). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Markosian, N. (2013). Time. In (E. N. Zalta, Ed.) The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/ archives/fall2013/entriesime/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2004). Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. Cognitive Brain Research, 21(2), 139–170. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.06.012 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T. (2001). How real is fictive motion? (Ph.D. Dissertation). University of California, Santa Cruz. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T. (2004a). The conceptual motivation of fictive motion. In G. Radden & K.-U. Panther (Eds.), Studies in Linguistic Motivation (pp. 221–248). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T. (2004b). Fictive motion as cognitive simulation. Memory & Cognition, 32(8), 1389–1400. doi:10.3758/BF03206329 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T. (2006). Depicting fictive motion in drawings. In J. Luchjenbroers (Ed.), Cognitive Linguistics Investigations (pp. 67–85). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T., Ramscar, M., & Boroditsky, L. (2005). On the Experiential Link Between Spatial and Temporal Language. Cognitive Science, 29(4), 655–664. doi:10.1207/ s15516709cog0000_17 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matlock, T., & Richardson, D. C. (2004). Do eye movements go with fictive motion? In K. Forbus, D. Gentner, & T. Regier (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 909–914). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Matsumoto, Y. (1996). Subjective motion and English and Japanese verbs. Cognitive Linguistics, 7(2), 183–226. doi:10.1515/cogl.1996.7.2.183 | pl_PL |
dc.references | May, R., Angel, E., & Ellenberger, H. F. (Eds.). (1994). Existence [First published in 1958]. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield. | pl_PL |
dc.references | McEnery, T., & Hardie, A. (2012). Corpus Linguistics: Method, Theory and Practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | McEnery, T., & Wilson, A. (2001). Corpus Linguistics: An Introduction, 2nd Ed. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | McEnery, T., Xiao, R., & Tono, Y. (2006). Corpus-Based Language Studies: An Advanced Resource Book. New York: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | McGlone, M. S. (2001). Concepts as Metaphors. In S. Glucksberg (Ed.), Understanding Figurative Language: From Metaphors to Idioms (pp. 90–107). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | McGlone, M. S. (2007). What is the explanatory value of a conceptual metaphor? Language & Communication, 27(2), 109–126. doi:10.1016/j.langcom.2006.02.016 | pl_PL |
dc.references | McGlone, M. S., & Harding, J. L. (1998). Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perscpective in temporal language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(5), 1211–1223. doi:10.1037/ 0278-7393.24.5.1211 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mcnamara, T. P., & Diwadkar, V. A. (1997). Symmetry and Asymmetry of Human Spatial Memory. Cognitive Psychology, 34(2), 160–190. doi:10.1006/cogp. 1997.0669 | pl_PL |
dc.references | McTaggart, J. E. (1908). The Unreality of Time. Mind, XVII(4), 457–474. doi:10.1093/mind/XVII.4.457 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Meck, W. H. (1996). Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. Cognitive Brain Research, 3(3-4), 227–242. doi:10.1016/0926-6410(96)00009-2 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945/1962). Phenomenology of Perception [First published in 1945 as Phénoménologie de la perception]. (C. Smith, Trans.). New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Merritt, D. J., Casasanto, D., & Brannon, E. M. (2010). Do monkeys think in metaphors? Representations of space and time in monkeys and humans. Cognition, 117(2), 191– 202. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.011 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Miles, L. K., Tan, L., Noble, G. D., Lumsden, J., & Macrae, C. N. (2011). Can a mind have two time lines? Exploring space–time mapping in Mandarin and English speakers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(3), 598–604. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0068-y | pl_PL |
dc.references | Millar, S. (2008). Space and Sense. Hove: Psychology Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Miller, G. A. (1995). WordNet: a lexical database for English. Communications of the ACM, 38(11), 39–41. doi:10.1145/219717.219748 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Miller, G. A., & Fellbaum, C. (2007). WordNet then and now. Language Resources and Evaluation, 41(2), 209–214. doi:10.1007/s10579-007-9044-6 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Miller, G. A., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1976). Language and Perception. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Minkowski, H. (1908/1964). Space and Time. [A translation of an address delivered at the 80th Assembly of German Natural Scientists and Physicians, at Cologne, September 21, 1908]. In J. J. C. Smart (Ed.), Problems of Space and Time. (pp. 297–312). New York: Macmillan. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Mix, K. S., Smith, L. B., & Gasser, M. (2010). The Spatial Foundations of Language and Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Montague, R. (1974). Formal Philosophy: Selected Papers of Richard Montague. (R. H. Thomason, Ed.). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Montello, D. R. (1991). The measurement of cognitive distance: Methods and construct validity. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 11(2), 101–122. doi:10.1016/ S0272-4944(05)80071-4 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Montello, D. R. (1993). Scale and multiple psychologies of space. In A. U. Frank & I. Campari (Eds.), Spatial Information Theory A Theoretical Basis for GIS: European Conference COSIT’93 Proceedings (pp. 312–321). Berlin: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Montello, D. R. (1997). The perception and cognition of environmental distance: Direct sources of information. In S. C. Hirtle & A. U. Frank (Eds.), Spatial Information Theory A Theoretical Basis for GIS: International Conference COSIT’97 Proceedings (pp. 297–311). Berlin: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Montello, D. R. (1998). A new framework for understanding the acquisition of spatial knowledge in large-scale environments. In M. J. Egenhofer & R. G. Golledge (Eds.), Spatial and Temporal Reasoning in Geographic Information Systems (pp. 143–154). New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Montello, D. R. (2009). A Conceptual Model of the Cognitive Processing of Environmental Distance Information. In K. S. Hornsby, C. Claramunt, M. Denis, & G. Ligozat (Eds.), Spatial Information Theory: International Conference COSIT 2009 Proceedings (Vol. 5756, pp. 1–17). Springer Berlin Heidelberg. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Moore, K. E. (2000). Spatial experience and temporal metaphors in Wolof: Point of view, conceptual mapping and linguistic practice. (Ph.D. Dissertation). University of California, Berkeley. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Moore, K. E. (2006). Space-to-time mappings and temporal concepts. Cognitive Linguistics, 17(2), 199–244. doi:10.1515/COG.2006.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Moore, K. E. (2011). Ego-perspective and field-based frames of reference: Temporal meanings of front in Japanese, Wolof, and Aymara. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(3), 759–776. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2010.07.003 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Morrow, D. G. (1985). Prominent characters and events organize narrative understanding. Journal of Memory and Language, 24(3), 304–319. doi:10.1016/0749- 596X(85)90030-0 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Morrow, D. G. (1990). Spatial models, prepositions, and verb-aspect markers. Discourse Processes, 13(4), 441–469. doi:10.1080/01638539009544769 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Morrow, D. G., & Clark, H. H. (1988). Interpreting words in spatial descriptions. Language and Cognitive Processes, 3(4), 275–291. doi:10.1080/ 01690968808402091 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Murphy, G. L. (1996). On metaphoric representation. Cognition, 60(2), 173–204. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(96)00711-1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Murphy, G. L. (1997). Reasons to doubt the present evidence for metaphoric representation. Cognition, 62(1), 99–108. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00725-1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Naylor-Emlen, S. J., & Taylor, H. A. (2009). Contributing Factors to Temporal and Spatial Associations in Mental Representations of Maps. Spatial Cognition & Computation, 9(4), 262–286. doi:10.1080/13875860903033472 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Newton, I. (1687/1995). The Principia: Mathematical Principles of Natural Philosophy [First published in 1687 as Philosophiae Naturalis Principia Mathematica]. (A. Motte, Trans.). Amherst, N.Y: Prometheus Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Noordzij, M. L., Zuidhoek, S., & Postma, A. (2006). The influence of visual experience on the ability to form spatial mental models based on route and survey descriptions. Cognition, 100(2), 321–342. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.05.006 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Núñez, R. E., & Cooperrider, K. (2013). The tangle of space and time in human cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(5), 220–229. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.008 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Núñez, R. E., Cooperrider, K., Doan, D., & Wassmann, J. (2012). Contours of time: Topographic construals of past, present, and future in the Yupno valley of Papua New Guinea. Cognition, 124(1), 25–35. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.007 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Núñez, R. E., & Cornejo, C. (2012). Facing the Sunrise: Cultural Worldview Underlying Intrinsic-Based Encoding of Absolute Frames of Reference in Aymara. Cognitive Science, 36(6), 965–991. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01237.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Núñez, R. E., Motz, B. A., & Teuscher, U. (2006). Time After Time: The Psychological Reality of the Ego- and Time-Reference-Point Distinction in Metaphorical Construals of Time. Metaphor and Symbol, 21(3), 133–146. doi:10.1207/ s15327868ms2103_1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Núñez, R. E., & Sweetser, E. (2006). With the Future Behind Them: Convergent Evidence From Aymara Language and Gesture in the Crosslinguistic Comparison of Spatial Construals of Time. Cognitive Science, 30(3), 401–450. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_62 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Nuttin, J. R. (1964). The future time perspective in human motivation and learning. Acta Psychologica, 23, 60–82. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(64)90075-7 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Nuttin, J. R. (1985). Future Time Perspective and Motivation: Theory and Research Method. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Nyaupane, G. P., Graefe, A. R., & Burns, R. C. (2003). Does Distance Matter? Differences in Characteristics, Behaviors and Attitudes of Visitors Based on Travel Distance. In J. J. Murdy (Ed.), Proceedings of the 2003 Northeastern Recreation Research Symposium (pp. 74–81). New York: United States Department of Agriculture. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ornstein, R. E. (1969). On the Experience of Time. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ornstein, R. E. (1972). The Psychology of Consciousness. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Israeli, Z., & Gabay, S. (2010). Is the Future the Right Time? Experimental Psychology, 57(4), 308–314. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000036 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Palmer, F. R. (2001). Mood and Modality, 2nd Ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Papafragou, A. (2010). Source-Goal Asymmetries in Motion Representation: Implications for Language Production and Comprehension. Cognitive Science, 34(6), 1064–1092. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01107.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Papafragou, A., Hulbert, J., & Trueswell, J. (2008). Does language guide event perception? Evidence from eye movements. Cognition, 108(1), 155–184. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.02.007 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Papafragou, A., Massey, C., & Gleitman, L. (2002). Shake, rattle, “n” roll: the representation of motion in language and cognition. Cognition, 84(2), 189–219. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00046-X | pl_PL |
dc.references | Papafragou, A., Massey, C., & Gleitman, L. (2006). When English proposes what Greek presupposes: The cross-linguistic encoding of motion events. Cognition, 98(3), B75– B87. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.05.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Papafragou, A., & Selimis, S. (2010). Event categorisation and language: A cross-linguistic study of motion. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(2), 224–260. doi:10.1080/01690960903017000 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pawelec, A. (2006). The death of metaphor. Studia Linguistica Universitatis Iagellonicae Cracoviensis, 123, 117–121. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pecher, D., & Zwaan, R. A. (Eds.). (2005). Grounding Cognition: The Role of Perception and Action in Memory, Language, and Thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Peirsman, Y., & Geeraerts, D. (2006). Metonymy as a prototypical category. Cognitive Linguistics, 17(3), 269–316. doi:10.1515/COG.2006.007 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pęzik, P. (2011). Providing corpus feedback for translators with the PELCRA search engine for NKJP. In S. Goźdź-Roszkowski (Ed.), Explorations Across Languages and Corpora: PALC 2009. (pp. 135–144). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pęzik, P. (2012a). Język mówiony w NKJP. In A. Przepiórkowski, M. Bańko, R. L. Górski, & B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Eds.), Narodowy Korpus Jezyka Polskiego (pp. 37– 47). Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pęzik, P. (2012b). Wyszukiwarka PELCRA dla danych NKJP. In A. Przepiórkowski, M. Bańko, R. L. Górski, & B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Eds.), Narodowy Korpus Jezyka Polskiego (pp. 253–279). Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piaget, J. (1946/1969). The Child’s Conception of Time [First published in 1946 as Le développement de la notion de temps chez l’enfant]. (A. J. Pomerans, Trans.). New York: Basic Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piaget, J. (1946/1970). The Child’s Conception of Movement and Speed [First published in 1946 as Les notions de mouvement et de vitesse chez l’enfant]. (G. E. T. Holloway & M. J. Mackenzie, Trans.). New York: Basic Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piaget, J. (1979). Relations Between Psychology and Other Sciences. Annual Review of Psychology, 30(1), 1–9. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.30.020179.000245 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1948/1956). The Child’s Conception of Space [First published in 1948 as La représentation de l’espace chez l’enfant]. (F. J. Langdon & J. L. Lunzer, Trans.). New York: Humanities Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., & Szeminska, A. (1948/1960). The Child’s Conception of Geometry [First published in 1948 as La géométrie spontanée de l’enfant]. (E. A. Lunzer, Trans.). New York: Basic Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Piasecki, M., Broda, B., & Szpakowicz, S. (2009). A Wordnet from the Ground Up. Wrocław: Oficyna Wydawnicza Politechniki Wrocławskiej. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pinker, S. (1990). A theory of graph comprehension. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.), Artificial Intelligence and the Future of Testing (pp. 73–126). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pinker, S. (2007a). The Stuff of Thought: Language as a Window into Human Nature. New York: Viking. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pinker, S. (2007b). The Discover interview Steven Pinker. DiscoverMagazine, (September, 2007), 48–52. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pinker, S., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of language: what’s special about it? Cognition, 95(2), 201–236. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pöppel, E. (2004). Lost in time: a historical frame, elementary processing units and the 3- second window. Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis, 64(3), 295–301. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pöppel, E. (2009). Pre-semantically defined temporal windows for cognitive processing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1887–1896. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0015 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Portugali, J. (1993). Implicate Relations: Society and Space in the Israeli-Palestinian Conflict. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Price-Williams, D. R. (1954). The Kappa Effect. Nature, 173(4399), 363–364. doi:10.1038/173363a0 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Prior, A. N. (1958). Time After Time. Mind, LXVII(266), 244–246. doi:10.1093/ mind/LXVII.266.244 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Przepiórkowski, A., Bańko, M., Górski, R. L., & Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (Eds.). (2012). Narodowy Korpus Jezyka Polskiego. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Pullum, G. K. (1991). The Great Eskimo Vocabulary Hoax, and Other Irreverent Essays on the Study of Language. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Quine, W. V. O. (1960/2013). Word and Object [First published in 1960]. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G. N., & Svartvik, J. (1985). A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. London: Longman. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Radden, G. (2004). The metaphor TIME AS SPACE across languages. In N. Baumarten, C. Böttger, M. Motz, & J. Probst (Eds.), Übersetzen, Interkulturelle Kommunikation, Spracherwerb und Sprachvermittlung – das Leben mit mehreren Sprachen: Festschrift für Juliane House zum 60. Geburtstag (pp. 225–238). Bochum: AKS-Verlag. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Radden, G., & Dirven, R. (2007). Cognitive English Grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Radvansky, G. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2011). Event perception. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(6), 608–620. doi:10.1002/wcs.133 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rakowski, W. (1986). Future Time Perspective: Applications to the Health Context of Later Adulthood. American Behavioral Scientist, 29(6), 730–745. doi:10.1177/000276486029006007 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ramachandran, V. S., & Anstis, S. M. (1986). The Perception of Apparent Motion. Scientific American, 254(6), 102–109. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0686-102 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rammsayer, T. H. (1997). On the relationship between personality and time estimation. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(5), 739–744. doi:10.1016/S0191- 8869(97)00117-7 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ramscar, M., Matlock, T., & Boroditsky, L. (2010). Time, Motion, and Meaning: The Experiential Basis of Abstract Thought. In K. S. Mix, L. B. Smith, & M. Gasser (Eds.), The Spatial Foundations of Language and Cognition (pp. 67–82). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rappaport Hovav, M., Doron, E., & Sichel, I. (Eds.). (2010). Lexical Semantics, Syntax, and Event Structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rasmussen, J. E. (Ed.). (1973/2007). Man in Isolation and Confinement [First published in 1973]. Piscataway, NJ: AldineTransaction. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rauh, G. (Ed.). (1983). Essays on deixis. Tübingen: Gunter Narr. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Reddy, M. J. (1979/1993). The conduit metaphor: A case of frame conflict in our language about language [First published in 1979]. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and Thought, 2nd Ed. (pp. 164–201). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Reines, M. F., & Prinz, J. (2009). Reviving Whorf: The Return of Linguistic Relativity. Philosophy Compass, 4(6), 1022–1032. doi:10.1111/j.1747-9991.2009.00260.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Richards, I. A. (1936). The Philosophy of Rhetoric. New York: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Richards, M. M., & Hawpe, L. S. (1981). Contrasting patterns in the acquisition of spatial/temporal terms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32(3), 485–512. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(81)90110-7 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Richardson, D., & Matlock, T. (2007). The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye movement study of fictive motion. Cognition, 102(1), 129–138. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.12.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rinck, M. (2005). Spatial Situation Models. In P. Shah & A. Miyake (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Visuospatial Thinking (pp. 334–382). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rinck, M., & Bower, G. H. (2000). Temporal and spatial distance in situation models. Memory & Cognition, 28(8), 1310–1320. doi:10.3758/BF03211832 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Riso, L. P., & McBride, C. (2007). Introduction: A Return to a Focus on Cognitive Schemas. In L. P. Riso, P. L. du Toit, D. J. Stein, & J. E. Young (Eds.), Cognitive Schemas and Core Beliefs in Psychological Problems: A Scientist-Practitioner Guide. (pp. 3–9). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ritchie, L. D. (2004). Lost in “Conceptual Space”: Metaphors of Conceptual Integration. Metaphor and Symbol, 19(1), 31–50. doi:10.1207/S15327868MS1901_2 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Roeckelein, J. E. (2000). The Concept of Time in Psychology: A Resource Book and Annotated Bibliography. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Roeckelein, J. E. (2008). History of Conceptions and Accounts of Time and Early Time Perception Research. In S. Grondin (Ed.), Psychology of Time (pp. 1–50). Bingley: Emerald. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rohrer, T. (2007). Embodiment and Experientialism. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 25–47). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rojo, A., & Valenzuela, J. (2009). Fictive Motion in Spanish: Travellable, non-travellable and path-related manner information. In J. Valenzuela, A. Rojo, & C. Soriano (Eds.), Trends in Cognitive Linguistics: Theoretical and Applied Models (pp. 244–260). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Roos, P., & Albers, R. (1965). Performance of alcoholics and normals on a measure of temporal orientation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21(1), 34–36. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rosch, E. (1975a). Cognitive reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 7(4), 532–547. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(75)90021-3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rosch, E. (1975b). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104(3), 192–233. doi:10.1037/0096- 3445.104.3.192 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of Categorisation. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and Categorisation (pp. 27–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Roussel, M.-È., Grondin, S., & Killeen, P. (2009). Spatial effects on temporal categorisation. Perception, 38(5), 748–762. doi:10.1068/p6165 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J., & Pérez Hernández, L. (2011). The Contemporary Theory of Metaphor: Myths, Developments and Challenges. Metaphor and Symbol, 26(3), 161–185. doi:10.1080/10926488.2011.583189 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Rynasiewicz, R. (2012). Newton’s Views on Space, Time, and Motion. In (E. N. Zalta, Ed.) The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Stanford, CA: Stanford University. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2012/entries/newton-stm/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sadalla, E. K., Burroughs, J. W., & Staplin, L. J. (1980). Reference points in spatial cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6(5), 516–528. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.6.5.516 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sandra, D., & Rice, S. (1995). Network analyses of prepositional meaning: Mirroring whose mind—the linguist’s or the language user’s? Cognitive Linguistics, 6(1), 89–130. doi:10.1515/cogl.1995.6.1.89 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Santiago, J., Lupáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 512–516. doi:10.3758/BF03194099 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sarrazin, J.-C., Giraudo, M.-D., Pailhous, J., & Bootsma, R. J. (2004). Dynamics of Balancing Space and Time in Memory: Tau and Kappa Effects Revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(3), 411–430. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.30.3.411 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sarrazin, J.-C., Tonnelier, A., & Alexandre, F. (2005). A model of contextual effect on reproduced extents in recall tasks: the issue of the imputed motion hypothesis. Biological Cybernetics, 92(5), 303–315. doi:10.1007/s00422-005-0553-3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sattig, T. (2006). The Language and Reality of Time. Oxford: Clarendon Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Saygin, A. P., McCullough, S., Alac, M., & Emmorey, K. (2010). Modulation of BOLD Response in Motion-sensitive Lateral Temporal Cortex by Real and Fictive Motion Sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(11), 2480–2490. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21388 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On Depression, Development, and Death. San Francisco, CA: W.H. Freeman. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sheets-Johnstone, M. (2011). The Primacy of Movement, Expanded 2nd Ed. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Shigeno, S. (1986). The auditory tau and kappa effects for speech and nonspeech stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 40(1), 9–19. doi:10.3758/BF03207588 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Simner, J. (2007). Beyond perception: synaesthesia as a psycholinguistic phenomenon. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 23–29. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.010 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinclair, J. (1991). Corpus, Concordance, Collocation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinclair, J. (1996). Preliminary recommendations on Corpus Typology. (EAGLES Guidelines 1996 No. EAG—TCWG—CTYP/P). Istituto di Linguistica Computazionale, Pisa: Expert Advisory Group on Language Engineering Standards. Retrieved from http://www.ilc.cnr.it/EAGLES/corpustyp/corpustyp.html | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinclair, J. (2003a). Reading Concordances: An Introduction. London: Pearson Longman. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinclair, J. (2003b). Corpus processing. In P. van Sterkenburg (Ed.), A Practical Guide to Lexicography (pp. 179–193). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinclair, J. (2004). Trust the Text: Language, Corpus and Discourse. London: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinclair, J. (2005). Corpus and Text — Basic Principles. In W. Martin (Ed.), Developing Linguistic Corpora: A Guide to Good Practice (pp. 1–16). Oxford: Oxbow Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sinha, C., Sinha, V. D. S., Zinken, J., & Sampaio, W. (2011). When time is not space: The social and linguistic construction of time intervals and temporal event relations in an Amazonian culture. Language and Cognition, 3(1), 137–169. doi:10.1515/ langcog.2011.006 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Skow, B. (2010). On the meaning of the question “How fast does time pass?” Philosophical Studies, 155(3), 325–344. doi:10.1007/s11098-010-9575-3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (1987). Thinking for Speaking. In Proceedings of the Thirteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 435–445). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (1996a). From “though and language” to “thinking for speaking”. In J. J. Gumperz & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking Linguistic Relativity (pp. 70–96). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (1996b). Two Ways to Travel: Verbs of Motion in English and Spanish. In M. Shibatani & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Grammatical Constructions: Their Form and Meaning (pp. 195–219). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (1997). Mind, Code, and Text. In J. L. Bybee, J. Haiman, & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Essays on Language Function and Language Type: Dedicated to T. Givón (pp. 437–467). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (2003). Language and thought online: Cognitive consequences of linguistic relativity. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought (pp. 157–192). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (2004). The Many Ways to Search for a Frog: Linguistic Typology and the Expression of Motion Events. In S. Strömqvist & L. T. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating Events in Narrative, Vol. 2: Typological and Contextual Perspectives (pp. 219–257). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I. (2006). What makes manner of motion salient? Explorations in linguistic typology, discourse, and cognition. In M. Hickmann & S. Robert (Eds.), Space in Languages: Linguistic Systems and Cognitive Categories (pp. 59–81). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Slobin, D. I., & Hoiting, N. (1994). Reference to Movement in Spoken and Signed Languages. In Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: General Session Dedicated to the Contributions of Charles J. Fillmore (pp. 487–505). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Smart, J. J. C. (1949). The River of Time. Mind, LVIII(232), 483–494. doi:10.1093/mind/LVIII.232.483 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Smart, J. J. C. (1955). Spatialising Time. Mind, LXIV(254), 239–241. doi:10.1093/mind/LXIV.254.239 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Smart, J. J. C. (2008). The Tenseless Theory of Time. In T. Sider, J. Hawthorne, & D. W. Zimmerman (Eds.), Contemporary Debates in Metaphysics (pp. 226–238). Malden, MA: Blackwell. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Smilek, D., Callejas, A., Dixon, M. J., & Merikle, P. M. (2007). Ovals of time: Time-space associations in synaesthesia. Consciousness and Cognition, 16(2), 507–519. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2006.06.013 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Somov, P. G. (2000). Time perception as a measure of pain intensity and pain type. Journal of Back and Musculoskeletal Rehabilitation, 14(3), 111–121. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sorabji, R. (1988). Matter, Space and Motion: Theories in Antiquity and Their Sequel. London: Duckworth. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Spence, C., & Driver, J. (Eds.). (2004). Crossmodal Space and Crossmodal Attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Spence, I. (2004). The Apparent and Effective Dimensionality of Representations of Objects. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 46(4), 738–747. doi:10.1518/hfes.46.4.738.56809 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Srinivasan, M., & Carey, S. (2010). The long and the short of it: On the nature and origin of functional overlap between representations of space and time. Cognition, 116(2), 217– 241. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.005 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stefanowitsch, A., & Rohde, A. (2004). The goal bias in the encoding of motion events. In G. Radden & K.-U. Panther (Eds.), Studies in Linguistic Motivation (pp. 249–267). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Steinman, R. M., Pizlo, Z., & Pizlo, F. J. (2000). Phi is not beta, and why Wertheimer’s discovery launched the Gestalt revolution. Vision Research, 40(17), 2257–2264. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00086-9 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 64(3), 153–181. doi:10.1037/h0046162 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stevens, S. S. (1969). On predicting exponents for cross-modality matches. Perception & Psychophysics, 6(4), 251–256. doi:10.3758/BF03207028 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stevens, S. S. (1986). Psychophysics: Introduction to Its Perceptual, Neural, and Social Prospects [First published in 1975]. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Strathman, A., Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., & Edwards, C. S. (1994). The consideration of future consequences: Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(4), 742–752. doi:10.1037/0022- 3514.66.4.742 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Stubbs, M. (1995). Collocations and semantic profiles: On the cause of the trouble with quantitative studies. Functions of Language, 2(1), 23–55. doi:10.1075/fol.2.1.03stu | pl_PL |
dc.references | Suddendorf, T. (2013). Mental time travel: continuities and discontinuities. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(4), 151–152. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.011 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(3). doi:10.1017/S0140525X07001975 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sullivan, J. L., & Barth, H. C. (2012). Active (not passive) spatial imagery primes temporal judgements. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(6), 1101–1109. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.641025 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Szeląg, E., & Pöppel, E. (Eds.). (2004). Time, Cognition, Thinking. [Special Issue]. Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis, 64(3). | pl_PL |
dc.references | Szwedek, A. (2002). Objectification: From object perception to metaphor creation. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk & K. Turewicz (Eds.), Cognitive Linguistics Today (pp. 159–175). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Szwedek, A. (2007). An alternative theory of metaphorization. In M. Fabiszak (Ed.), Language and Meaning: Cognitive and Functional Perspectives (pp. 313–327). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Szwedek, A. (2008). Ontological sources of structural and orientational metaphors. In Z. Wąsik & T. Komendziński (Eds.), Metaphor and Cognition (pp. 9–16). Frankfurt am Mein: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Szwedek, A. (2009a). Conceptualization of space and time. In P. Łobacz, W. Zabrocki, & P. Nowak (Eds.), Language, Science and Culture: Essays in Honor of Professor Jerzy Bańczerowski on the Occasion of His 70th Birthday (pp. 317–333). Poznan: Wydawnictwo Naukowe UAM. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Szwedek, A. (2009b). Are prepositions metaphorical? In W. Oleksy & P. Stalmaszczyk (Eds.), Cognitive Approaches to Language and Linguistic Data: Studies of Honor of Barbara Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (pp. 171–180). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Szwedek, A. (2011). The ultimate source domain. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 9(2), 341–366. doi:10.1075/rcl.9.2.01szw | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tabakowska, E. (2003). Space and Time in Polish: The Preposition za and the Verbal Prefix za-. In H. Cuyckens, T. Berg, R. Dirven, & K.-U. Panther (Eds.), Motivation in Language: Studies in Honor of Günter Radden (pp. 153–177). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1975). Figure and Ground in Complex Sentences. In Proceedings of the First Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 419–430). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1983). How language structures space. In H. L. Pick & L. P. Acredolo (Eds.), Spatial Orientation: Theory, Research, and Application (pp. 225–282). New York: Plenum Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1985). Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language Typology and Syntactic Description, Vol. 3: Grammatical Categories and the Lexicon (pp. 57–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1988a). Force Dynamics in Language and Cognition. Cognitive Science, 12(1), 49–100. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1201_2 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1988b/2007). The relation of grammar to cognition [First published in 1988]. In V. Evans, B. K. Bergen, & J. Zinken (Eds.), The Cognitive Linguistics Reader (pp. 481–544). London: Equinox. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1991). Path to Realization: A Typology of Event Conflation. In Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: General Session and Parasession on The Grammar of Event Structure (pp. 480–519). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (1996). Fictive Motion in Language and “Ception.” In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and Space (pp. 211–276). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2000a). Toward a Cognitive Semantics, Vol. I: Concept Structuring Systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2000b). Toward a Cognitive Semantics, Vol. II: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2005a). The fundamental system of spatial schemas in language. In B. Hampe (Ed.), From Perception to Meaning: Image Schemas in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 199–234). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2005b). Leonard Talmy. A windowing to conceptual structure and language, Part 1: Lexicalisation and typology [Interview by Iraide Ibarretxe-Antuñano]. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 3(1), 325–347. doi:10.1075/arcl.3.17iba | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2007). Foreword [comparing introspection with other methodologies]. In M. Gonzalez-Marquez, I. Mittelberg, S. Coulson, & M. J. Spivey (Eds.), Methods in Cognitive Linguistics (pp. xi–xxi). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2009). Main Verb Properties and Equipollent Framing. In J. Guo, E. Lieven, N. Budwig, S. Ervin-Tripp, K. Nakamura, & Ş. Özçalişkan (Eds.), Crosslinguistic Approaches to the Psychology of Language: Research in the Tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin (pp. 389–402). New York: Psychology Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2010, October). The Mind and the Strangest of Languages. Ciencia Hoje [Science Today, Brazil], 46(275), 88–10. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Talmy, L. (2011). Cognitive Semantics: An overview. In C. Maienborn, K. von Heusinger, & P. Portner (Eds.), Semantics: An International Handbook of Natural Language Meaning (Vol. 1, pp. 622–642). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tarski, A. (1944). The Semantic Conception of Truth: And the Foundations of Semantics. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 4(3), 341–376. doi:10.2307/2102968 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tatarkiewicz, W. (2005). Historia filozofii. T. 1: Filozofia starożytna i średniowieczna. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1992a). Spatial mental models derived from survey and route descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 31(2), 261–292. doi:10.1016/0749- 596X(92)90014-O | pl_PL |
dc.references | Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1992b). Descriptions and depictions of environments. Memory & Cognition, 20(5), 483–496. doi:10.3758/BF03199581 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1996). Perspective in Spatial Descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(3), 371–391. doi:10.1006/jmla.1996.0021 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tenbrink, T. (2007). Space, Time, and the Use of Language: An Investigation of Relationships. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tenbrink, T. (2011). Reference frames of space and time in language. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(3), 704–722. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2010.06.020 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tenny, C., & Pustejovsky, J. (2000). A History of Events in Linguistic Theory. In C. Tenny & J. Pustejovsky (Eds.), Events as Grammatical Objects: The Converging Perspectives of Lexical Semantics and Syntax (pp. 3–37). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ter Meulen, A. G. B. (1995). Representing Time in Natural Language: The Dynamic Interpretation of Tense and Aspect. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Teubert, W. (2005). My version of corpus linguistics. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 10(1), 1–13. doi:10.1075/ijcl.10.1.01teu | pl_PL |
dc.references | Teuscher, U., McQuire, M., Collins, J., & Coulson, S. (2008). Congruity Effects in Time and Space: Behavioral and ERP Measures. Cognitive Science, 32(3), 563–578. doi:10.1080/03640210802035084 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Thelen, E. (1995). Motor Development: A New Synthesis. American Psychologist, 50(2), 79–95. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.50.2.79 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Teuscher, U., McQuire, M., Collins, J., & Coulson, S. (2008). Congruity Effects in Time and Space: Behavioral and ERP Measures. Cognitive Science, 32(3), 563–578. doi:10.1080/03640210802035084 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Thelen, E. (1995). Motor Development: A New Synthesis. American Psychologist, 50(2), 79–95. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.50.2.79 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Therriault, D. J., & Rinck, M. (2007). Multidimensional situation models. In F. Schmalhofer & C. A. Perfetti (Eds.), Higher Level Language Processes in the Brain: Inference and Comprehension Processes (pp. 311–327). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Thompson, P. (2005). Spoken Language Corpora. In W. Martin (Ed.), Developing Linguistic Corpora: A Guide to Good Practice (pp. 59–70). Oxford: Oxbow Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Thorndyke, P. W., & Hayes-Roth, B. (1982). Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation. Cognitive Psychology, 14(4), 560–589. doi:10.1016/0010- 0285(82)90019-6 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Toda, M. (1978). The boundaries of the notion of time. In J. T. Fraser, N. Lawrence, & D. Park (Eds.), The Study of Time III: Proceedings of the Third Conference of the International Society for the Study of Time (pp. 370–388). Berlin: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tognini-Bonelli, E. (2001). Corpus Linguistics at Work. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive Maps in Rats and Men. Psychological Review, 55(4), 189– 208. doi:10.1037/h0061626 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tommasi, L., & Laeng, B. (2012). Psychology of spatial cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(6), 565–580. doi:10.1002/wcs.1198 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2006). Flexible Conceptual Projection of Time Onto Spatial Frames of Reference. Cognitive Science, 30(4), 745–757. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_67 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Traugott, E. C. (1975). Spatial Expressions of Tense and Temporal Sequencing: A Contribution to the Study of Semantic Felds. Semiotica, 15(3). doi:10.1515/ semi.1975.15.3.207 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Traugott, E. C. (1978). On the expression of spatio-temporal relations in language. In J. H. Greenberg, C. A. Ferguson, & E. A. Moravcsik (Eds.), Universals of Human Language, Vol. 3: Word Structure. (pp. 369–400). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 77(13), 1–31. doi:10.1037/h0093864 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Trenga, B. (2010, October 7). “Think Different” or “Think Differently”: Can Emmy-awardwinning advertisement be ungrammatical? Grammar Girl. Retrieved from http://www.quickanddirtytips.com/education/grammar/think-different-or-thinkdifferently | pl_PL |
dc.references | Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110(3), 403– 421. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440–463. doi:10.1037/a0018963 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tuggy, D. (1993). Ambiguity, polysemy, and vagueness. Cognitive Linguistics, 4(3), 273– 290. doi:10.1515/cogl.1993.4.3.273 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tulving, E. (2002). Chronesthesia: Conscious Awareness of Subjective Time. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of Frontal Lobe Function (pp. 311–325). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tummers, J., Heylen, K., & Geeraerts, D. (2005). Usage-based approaches in Cognitive Linguistics: A technical state of the art. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 1(2), 225–261. doi:10.1515/cllt.2005.1.2.225 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Turewicz, K. (2005). Understanding prepositions through Cognitive Grammar: A case of in. In K. Turewicz (Ed.), Cognitive Linguistics: A User-Friendly Approach (pp. 103– 122). Szczecin: Wydawnictwo Naukowe Uniwersytetu Szczecińskiego. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Turner, F., & Pöppel, E. (1983). The Neural Lyre: Poetic Meter, the Brain, and Time. Poetry, 142, 277–309. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Turner, M. (2007). Conceptual Integration. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 377–393). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tutton, M. (2012). Granularity, space, and motion-framed location. In M. Vulchanova & E. van der Zee (Eds.), Motion Encoding in Language and Space (pp. 149–165). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124–1131. doi:10.1126/science.185.4157.1124 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (1993). Cognitive maps, cognitive collages, and spatial mental models. In A. U. Frank & I. Campari (Eds.), Spatial Information Theory: A Theoretical Basis for GIS: European Conference, COSIT’93 Proceedings (pp. 14–24). Berlin: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2000). Remembering Spaces. In E. Tulving & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2003). Structures Of Mental Spaces: How People Think About Space. Environment & Behavior, 35(1), 66–80. doi:10.1177/0013916502238865 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2004). Narratives of Space, Time, and Life. Mind and Language, 19(4), 380– 392. doi:10.1111/j.0268-1064.2004.00264.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2005a). Visuospatial Reasoning. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning (pp. 209–240). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2005b). How to get around by mind and body: spatial thought, spatial action. In A. Zilhão (Ed.), Evolution, Rationality, and Cognition: A Cognitive Science for the Twenty-First Century (pp. 135–147). London: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2009). Spatial Cognition: Embodied and Situated. In P. Robbins & M. Aydede (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition (pp. 201–216). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B. (2011). Visualizing Thought. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3(3), 499–535. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01113.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B., & Hard, B. M. (2009). Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking. Cognition, 110(1), 124–129. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.008 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B., Kugelmass, S., & Winter, A. (1991). Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology, 23(4), 515–557. doi:10.1016/0010- 0285(91)90005-9 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tversky, B., & Lee, P. U. (1998). How Space Structures Language. In C. Freksa, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial Cognition: An Interdisciplinary Approach to Representing and Processing Spatial Knowledge (pp. 157–175). Berlin: Springer. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tyler, A., & Evans, V. (2001). Reconsidering Prepositional Polysemy Networks: The Case of over. Language, 77(4), 724–765. doi:10.1353/lan.2001.0250 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Tyler, A., & Evans, V. (2003). The Semantics of English Prepositions Spatial Scenes, Embodied Meaning, and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Vandeloise, C. (1990). Representation, prototypes and centrality. In S. L. Tsohatzidis (Ed.), Meanings and Prototypes: Studies in Linguistic Categorization (pp. 403–437). London: Routledge. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Vandeloise, C. (1991). Spatial Prepositions: A Case Study from French. (A. R. K. Bosch, Trans.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Vandeloise, C. (1994). Methodology and analyses of the preposition in. Cognitive Linguistics, 5(2), 157–184. doi:10.1515/cogl.1994.5.2.157 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Verkuyl, H. J. (1993). A Theory of Aspectuality: The Interaction Between Temporal and Atemporal Structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Von Wright, G. H. (1969). Time, Change and Contradiction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Vossen, P. (2002). EuroWordNet: General Document (Final Report Version 3). Amsterdam: University of Amsterdam. Retrieved from http://vossen.info/ docs/2002/EWNGeneral.pdf | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wackermann, J. (2007). Inner and Outer Horizons of Time Experience. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 10(1), 20–32. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wagner, M. (2006). The Geometries of Visual Space. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (in press). Warunek instrumentu dla wyrażeń ruchu fiktywnego. In ZBLIŻENIA: językoznawstwo - translatoryka - literaturoznawstwo. Konin: Wydawnictwo PWSZ w Koninie. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2005a). Typologia korpusów oraz warsztat informatyczny lingwistyki korpusowej. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Podstawy jezykoznawstwa korpusowego (pp. 27–41). Łódź: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2005b). Korpusy wielojęzyczne - równoległe i porównywalne. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Podstawy jezykoznawstwa korpusowego (pp. 42– 60). Łódź: Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2012a). Spatial and temporal representations of topographical distance in the British National Corpus (COST TIMELY Research Report No. 03/2012). Lodz: University of Lodz. Retrieved from http://anglistyka.uni.lodz.pl/userfiles/ timely/BNC-Away.pdf | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2012b). Spatial and temporal representations of distance in contexts paralel to “away” in the National Corpus of Polish (COST TIMELY Research Report No. 04/2012). Lodz: University of Lodz. Retrieved from http://anglistyka.uni.lodz.pl/userfiles/timely/NCP-Away.pdf | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2012c). References to temporal perspective in the PELCRA Spoken Conversational Corpus (COST TIMELY Research Report No. 02/2012). Lodz: University of Lodz. Retrieved from http://anglistyka.uni.lodz.pl/userfiles/ timely/PELCRA-SCCP-TemporalPerspective.pdf | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2013a). Representations of motion-framed distance in spatial and temporal terms in the British National Corpus (COST TIMELY Research Report No. 02/2013). Lodz: University of Lodz. Retrieved from http://anglistyka.uni.lodz.pl/ userfiles/timely/BNC-MotionFramedDistance.pdf | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2013b). Representations of motion-framed distance in spatial and temporal terms in the National Corpus of Polish (COST TIMELY Research Report No. 03/2013). Lodz: University of Lodz. Retrieved from http://anglistyka.uni.lodz.pl/ userfiles/timely/NCP-MotionFramedDistance.pdf | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2013c). Temporality of coextension path expressions in the British National Corpus (COST TIMELY Research Report No. 04/2013). Lodz: University of Lodz. Retrieved from http://anglistyka.uni.lodz.pl/userfiles/ timely/BNC-CoextensionPathTemporality.pdf | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T. (2013d). Temporality of coextension path expressions in the National Corpus of Polish (COST TIMELY Research Report No. 05/2013). Lodz: University of Lodz. Retrieved from http://anglistyka.uni.lodz.pl/userfiles/ timely/NCP-CoextensionPathTemporality.pdf | pl_PL |
dc.references | Waliński, J. T., & Pęzik, P. (2007). Web access interface to the PELCRA referential corpus of Polish. In J. T. Waliński, K. Kredens, & S. Goźdź-Roszkowski (Eds.), Corpora and ICT in language studies: PALC 2005 (pp. 65–86). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wallace, M. (1956). Future time perspective in schizophrenia. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52(2), 240–245. doi:10.1037/h0039899 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wallentin, M., Ostergaard, S., Lund, T., Ostergaard, L., & Roepstorff, A. (2005). Concrete spatial language: See what I mean? Brain and Language, 92(3), 221–233. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.06.106 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483–488. doi:10.1016/ j.tics.2003.09.002 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Walsh, W. H. (1967). Kant on the Perception of Time. The Monist, 51(3), 376–396. doi:10.5840/monist196751323 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Warren, H. C. (Ed.). (1934). Dictionary of Psychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wearden, J. H., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (1995). Feeling the Heat: Body Temperature and the Rate of Subjective Time, Revisited. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 48(2), 129–141. doi:10.1080/14640749508401443 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wertheimer, M. (1912). Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen von Bewegung [Experimental Studies in the Perception of Movement]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 61, 161–265. | pl_PL |
dc.references | White, K. (1994). Aquinas, St. Thomas (1224/25–1274). In (S. L. Macey, Ed.) Encyclopedia of Time. New York: Garland. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Whorf, B. L. (1939/1956a). The Relation of Habitual Thought and Behavior to Language [Written in 1939]. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 134–159). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Whorf, B. L. (1940/1956b). Science and Linguistics [Written in 1940]. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf (pp. 207–219). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wierzbicka, A. (1996). Semantics: Primes and Universals. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Williams, J. M. (1976). Synaesthetic Adjectives: A Possible Law of Semantic Change. Language, 52(2), 461–478. doi:10.2307/412571 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Williams, R. F. (2004). Making meaning from a clock: Material artifacts and conceptual blending in time-telling instruction. (Ph.D. Dissertation). University of California, San Diego. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wilson, R. A., & Foglia, L. (2011). Embodied Cognition. In (E. N. Zalta, Ed.) The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/ archives/fall2011/entries/embodied-cognition/ | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wittgenstein, L. (1938/2003). Lectures and Conversations on Aesthetics, Psychology, and Religious Belief [First published in 1938]. (C. Barrett, Ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wittmann, M. (2011). Moments in Time. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 5, 66. doi:10.3389/fnint.2011.00066 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wittmann, M. (2013). The inner sense of time: how the brain creates a representation of duration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(3), 217–223. doi:10.1038/nrn3452 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wittmann, M., & Paulus, M. P. (2009). Temporal horizons in decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 2(1), 1–11. doi:10.1037/a0015460 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wittmann, M., & Pöppel, E. (2000). Temporal Mechanisms of the Brain as Fundamentals of Communication — With Special Reference to Music Perception and Performance. Musicae Scientiae, 3(1 suppl), 13–28. doi:10.1177/10298649000030S103 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wittmann, M., & van Wassenhove, V. (Eds.). (2009). The experience of time: neural mechanisms and the interplay of emotion, cognition and embodiment [Theme Issue]. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525). | pl_PL |
dc.references | Woelert, P. (2011). Human cognition, space, and the sedimentation of meaning. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 10(1), 113–137. doi:10.1007/s11097-010- 9153-3 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wohlford, P. (1966). Extension of personal time, affective states, and expectation of personal death. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3(5), 559–566. doi:10.1037/h0023208 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wolff, P., & Gentner, D. (2011). Structure-Mapping in Metaphor Comprehension. Cognitive Science, 35(8), 1456–1488. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01194.x | pl_PL |
dc.references | Wolff, P., & Holmes, K. J. (2011). Linguistic relativity. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(3), 253–265. doi:10.1002/wcs.104 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Xiao, R. (2008). Well-known and influential corpora. In A. Lüdeling & M. Kytö (Eds.), Corpus linguistics: an international handbook (pp. 383–457). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Yeh, W., & Barsalou, L. W. (2006). The Situated Nature of Concepts. The American Journal of Psychology, 119(3), 349. doi:10.2307/20445349 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zacks, J. M., Levy, E., Tversky, B., & Schiano, D. J. (1998). Reading bar graphs: Effects of extraneous depth cues and graphical context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4(2), 119–138. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.4.2.119 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zacks, J. M., & Tversky, B. (2012). Granularity in taxonomy, time, and space. In M. Vulchanova & E. van der Zee (Eds.), Motion Encoding in Language and Space (pp. 123–133). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1997). Temporal Cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6(1), 12–16. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512604 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zaleski, Z. (1996). Future Anxiety: concept, measurement, and preliminary research. Personality and Individual Differences, 21(2), 165–174. doi:10.1016/0191- 8869(96)00070-0 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zimbardo, P. G. (2002). Time to Take out Time. Psychology Today, 35, 62. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271–1288. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1271 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (2008). The Time Paradox: The New Psychology of Time That Will Change Your Life. New York: Free Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zimbardo, P. G., Keough, K. A., & Boyd, J. N. (1997). Present time perspective as a predictor of risky driving. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(6), 1007–1023. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00113-X | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zinken, J. (2010). Temporal frames of reference. In V. Evans & P. A. Chilton (Eds.), Language, Cognition and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions (pp. 479– 498). London: Equinox. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zlatev, J. (2007). Spatial Semantics. In D. Geeraerts & H. Cuyckens (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics (pp. 318–350). Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zlatev, J., Blomberg, J., & Magnusson, U. (2012). Metaphor and subjective experience: A study of motion-emotion metaphors in English, Swedish, Bulgarian, and Thai. In A. Foolen, U. M. Lüdtke, T. P. Racine, & J. Zlatev (Eds.), Moving Ourselves, Moving Others: Motion and emotion in intersubjectivity, consciousness and language. (pp. 423–450). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zlatev, J., & Yangklang, P. (2004). A Third Way to Travel: The Place of Thai in Motion- Event Typology. In S. Strömqvist & L. T. Verhoeven (Eds.), Relating Events in Narrative, Vol. 2: Typological and Contextual Perspectives (pp. 159–190). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral Expressions and Biosocial Bases of Sensation Seeking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A. (2004). The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 44 (pp. 35–62). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A. (2009). Time in Language, Situation Models, and Mental Simulations. In P. Indefrey & M. Gullberg (Eds.), Time to Speak: Cognitive and Neural Prerequisites for Time in Language (pp. 13–26). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A., Madden, C. J., & Stanfield, R. A. (2001). Time in narrative comprehension: A cognitive perspective. In D. H. Schram & G. Steen (Eds.), The Psychology and Sociology of Literature: In Honor of Elrud Ibsch (pp. 71–86). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A., Madden, C. J., Yaxley, R. H., & Aveyard, M. E. (2004). Moving words: dynamic representations in language comprehension. Cognitive Science, 28(4), 611– 619. doi:10.1016/j.cogsci.2004.03.004 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A., & Pecher, D. (2012). Revisiting Mental Simulation in Language Comprehension: Six Replication Attempts. PLoS ONE, 7(12), e51382. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0051382 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123(2), 162–185. doi:10.1037/0033- 2909.123.2.162 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Zwaan, R. A., & Taylor, L. J. (2006). Seeing, Acting, Understanding: Motor Resonance in Language Comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135(1), 1– 11. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.1 | pl_PL |
dc.references | Bryson, B. (2006). The Life and Times of the Thunderbolt Kid: A Memoir. New York: Broadway Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Eco, U. (1984). The Name of the Rose. (W. Weaver, Trans.). New York: Warner Books. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Jones, J. (1951). From Here to Eternity. New York: Scribner. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Marvell, A. (1999). To His Coy Mistress [Poem first published in 1681, anthology first published in 1939]. In A. Quiller-Couch (Ed.), The Oxford Book of English Verse, 1250–1918 (p. 357). Oxford: Clarendon Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Sand, G. (1991). Story of My Life: The Autobiography of George Sand. (Group Trans., T. Jurgrau, Ed.). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | The British National Corpus (2001). [World Edition] Available from OUCS at: www.natcorp.ox.ac.uk | pl_PL |
dc.references | The National Corpus of Polish (2010). Available at: www.nkjp.pl | pl_PL |
dc.references | The PELCRA Spoken Conversational Corpus of Polish (2010). Available at: www.nkjp.uni.lodz.pl/spoken.jsp | pl_PL |
dc.references | American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, 4th Edition. (2006). Boston: Houghton Mifflin. | pl_PL |
dc.references | Oxford English Dictionary, 2nd Edition, Version 4.0 (2009). [CD-ROM]. Oxford: Oxford University Press. | pl_PL |
dc.references | The PWN-Oxford English-Polish and Polish-English Dictionary (2004). [CD-ROM]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN. | pl_PL |
dc.references | R. (2012). A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available for download at: www.R-project.org | pl_PL |
dc.references | PELCRA web-based interface to the National Corpus of Polish (2010). A concordancer with support for proximity queries for the National Corpus of Polish. [Developed by Piotr Pęzik]. Łódź: University of Łódź. Available at: www.nkjp.uni.lodz.pl | pl_PL |
dc.references | SlopeQ. (2012). A part-of-speech-sensitive concordancer with support for proximity queries for the British National Corpus. [Developed by Piotr Pęzik]. Łódź: University of Łódź. | pl_PL |
dc.references | SłowoSieć Online Search 2.1. (2012). A lexical database for Polish with mappings between Polish WordNet and English [Princeton] WordNet. Wrocław: Wrocław University of Technology. Available at: plwordnet.pwr.wroc.pl | pl_PL |
dc.references | WordNet Online Search 3.1. (2012). A lexical database for English. Princeton: Princeton University. Available at: wordnetweb.princeton.edu/perl/webwn | pl_PL |
dc.references | Ishihara, M., Keller, P., Rossetti, Y., & Prinz, W. (2008). Horizontal spatial representations
of time: Evidence for the STEARC effect. Cortex, 44(4), 454–461.
doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.010 | |
dc.references | Lucy, J. A. (1992b). Grammatical Categories and Cognition: A Case Study of the Linguistic
Relativity Hypothesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. | |
dc.identifier.doi | 10.18778/7969-441-9 | |