Pokaż uproszczony rekord

dc.contributor.authorSchwartz, Geoffreyen
dc.contributor.authorBalas, Annaen
dc.contributor.authorRojczyk, Arkadiuszen
dc.date.accessioned2015-06-12T12:38:02Z
dc.date.available2015-06-12T12:38:02Z
dc.date.issued2014-06-26en
dc.identifier.issn1731-7533en
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/11089/9697
dc.description.abstractAlthough there is little consensus on the relevance of non-contrastive allophonic processes in L2 speech acquisition, EFL pronunciation textbooks cover the suppression of stop release in coda position. The tendency for held stops in English is in stark opposition to a number of other languages, including Polish, in which plosive release is obligatory. This paper presents phonetic data on the acquisition of English unreleased stops by Polish learners. Results show that in addition to showing a tendency for the target language pattern of unreleased plosives, advanced learners may acquire more native-like VC formant transitions. From the functional perspective, languages with unreleased stops may be expected to have robust formant patterns on the final portion of the preceding vowel, which allow listeners to identify the final consonant when it lacks an audible release burst (see e.g. Wright 2004). From the perspective of syllabic positions, it may be said that ‘coda’ stops are obligatorily released in Polish, yet may be unreleased in English. Thus, the traditional term ‘coda’ is insufficient to describe the prosodic properties of post-vocalic stops in Polish and English. These differences may be captured in the Onset Prominence framework (Schwartz 2013). In languages with unreleased stops, the mechanism of submersion places post-vocalic stops at the bottom of the representational hierarchy where they may be subject to weakening. Submersion produces larger prosodic constituents and thus has phonological consequences beyond ‘coda’ behavior.en
dc.publisherWydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiegoen
dc.relation.ispartofseriesResearch in Language;12en
dc.rightsThis work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 3.0 License.en
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/en
dc.subjectcoda stop releaseen
dc.subjectL2 speechen
dc.subjectphonetics-phonologyen
dc.titleStop Release in Polish English — Implications for Prosodic Constituencyen
dc.page.number131-144en
dc.contributor.authorAffiliationSchwartz Geoffrey - Faculty of English, Adam Mickiewicz University in Poznańen
dc.contributor.authorAffiliationBalas Anna - Faculty of English, Adam Mickiewicz University in Poznańen
dc.contributor.authorAffiliationRojczyk Arkadiusz - Institute of English, University of Silesiaen
dc.identifier.eissn2083-4616
dc.referencesArsenault, Paul. 2009. Coronal features and retroflexion in Dhivehi and other Indo-Aryan languages. Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics 30. 17–33.en
dc.referencesBergier, M. 2010. The occurrence of unreleased oral stops in English voice agreeing plosive clusters straddling word boundaries. Production experiments with Polish advanced learners of English. M. A. Thesis. University of Silesia.en
dc.referencesBest, C. T. 1995. A direct realist perspective on cross-language speech perception. In W. Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 171-204). Timonium, MD: York Press.en
dc.referencesBest, C. T., & Tyler, M. D. 2007. Nonnative and second-language speech perception: Commonalities and complementarities. In M. J. Munro & O.-S. Bohn (Eds), Second language speech learning – the role of language experience in speech perception and production (pp. 13-34). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.en
dc.referencesBoersma, P. & Weenink, D. 2011. Praat: doing phonetics by computer. [Computer program].Version 5.2.18.en
dc.referencesBogacka, A. 2007. Repopulating vowel space: English diphthong production by Polish learners of English. PhD dissertation. Adam Mickiwicz University in Poznań.en
dc.referencesCook, A. 2000. American Accent Training. Hauppauge, NY: Barron’s.en
dc.referencesCruttenden, A. 2001. Gimson’s Pronunciation of English(6th ed.). London: Arnold.en
dc.referencesDukiewicz, L. & I. Sawicka. 1995. Gramatyka współczesnego języka polskiego – fonetyka i fonologia [Grammar of modern Polish – phonetics and phonology]. Krakow: Wydawnictwo Instytutu Języka Polskiego PAN.en
dc.referencesFlege, J.E. 1995. Second language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In W.Strange (Ed.), Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research (pp. 233–277). Baltimore: York Press.en
dc.referencesJenkins, J. 2000. The phonology of English as an international language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.en
dc.referencesKahn, D. 1976. Syllable-based generalizations in English phonology. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.en
dc.referencesKang, Y. 2003. Perceptual similarity in loanword adaptation: English post-vocalic word-final stops to Korean. Phonology 20 (2): 219-273. doi: 10.1017/S0952675703004524en
dc.referencesKang, Y. 2011. Loanword phonology. In van Oostendorp, Marc, Colin Ewen, Elizabeth Hume, and Keren Rice, eds., Companion to Phonology. Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 2258-2282.en
dc.referencesLindblom, B. 1990. Explaining phonetic variation: a sketch of the H&H theory. In Speech Production and Speech Modelling, edited by W. J. Hardcastle and A. Marchal (The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic), pp. 403-439.en
dc.referencesRojczyk, A., A. Porzuczek & M. Bergier. 2013. Immediate and distracted imitation in second language speech: unreleased plosives in English. Research in Language 11.1. 3-18. DOI: doi: 10.2478/v10015-012-0007-7en
dc.referencesSchwartz, G. 2013. A representational parameter for onsetless syllables. Journal of Linguistics, 49 (3), 613-646. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/. doi: 10.1017/S0022226712000436 ThomsonISI: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=000325851300003&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=b7bc2757938ac7a7a821505f8243d9f3en
dc.referencesSteriade, D. 1993. Closure, release, and nasal contours. In M. K. Huffman and R. A. Krakow (Eds.). Nasals, nasalization, and the velum (pp. 401−470). San Diego: Academic Press.en
dc.referencesWright, R. 2004. Perceptual cue robustness and phonotactic constraints. In Hayes, B., R. Kirchner & D. Steriade (eds). Phonetically Based Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 34-57.en
dc.contributor.authorEmailSchwartz Geoffrey - geoff@ifa.amu.edu.plen
dc.contributor.authorEmailBalas Anna - abalas@wa.amu.edu.plen
dc.contributor.authorEmailRojczyk Arkadiusz - arkadiusz.rojczyk@us.edu.plen
dc.identifier.doi10.2478/rela-2014-0006en


Pliki tej pozycji

Thumbnail

Pozycja umieszczona jest w następujących kolekcjach

Pokaż uproszczony rekord

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 3.0 License.
Poza zaznaczonymi wyjątkami, licencja tej pozycji opisana jest jako This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 3.0 License.