Pokaż uproszczony rekord

dc.contributor.authorGärdenfors, Peter
dc.contributor.editorŁukowski, Piotr
dc.contributor.editorGemel, Aleksander
dc.contributor.editorŻukowski, Bartosz
dc.date.accessioned2020-10-08T11:36:19Z
dc.date.available2020-10-08T11:36:19Z
dc.date.issued2015
dc.identifier.citationGärdenfors P., Cognitive science: From computers to ant hills as models of human thought, [w:] Cognition, Meaning and Action. Lodz-Lund Studies in Cognitive Science, Łukowski P., Gemel A., Żukowski B. (red.), Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego, Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Jagiellońskiego, Łódź-Kraków 2015, s. 11-30, doi: 10.18778/7969-759-5.02pl_PL
dc.identifier.isbn978-83-7969-759-5
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/11089/32311
dc.description.sponsorshipUdostępnienie publikacji Wydawnictwa Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego finansowane w ramach projektu „Doskonałość naukowa kluczem do doskonałości kształcenia”. Projekt realizowany jest ze środków Europejskiego Funduszu Społecznego w ramach Programu Operacyjnego Wiedza Edukacja Rozwój; nr umowy: POWER.03.05.00-00-Z092/17-00.pl_PL
dc.language.isoenpl_PL
dc.publisherWydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego, Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Jagiellońskiegopl_PL
dc.relation.ispartofŁukowski P., Gemel A., Żukowski B. (red.), Cognition, Meaning and Action. Lodz-Lund Studies in Cognitive Science, Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego, Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Jagiellońskiego, Łódź-Kraków 2015;
dc.rightsAttribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Międzynarodowe*
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/*
dc.subjectcognitive sciencepl_PL
dc.subjecthuman thoughtpl_PL
dc.titleCognitive science: From computers to ant hills as models of human thoughtpl_PL
dc.typeBook chapterpl_PL
dc.page.number11-30pl_PL
dc.identifier.eisbn978-83-7969-760-1
dc.contributor.authorBiographicalnotePeter Gärdenfors is Professor of Cognitive Science at Lund University since 1988. His main research interests concern concept formation, concept learning, the semantics of natural language and the evolution of cognition. His main books are Knowledge in Flux, Bradford Books, MIT Press, 1988 (second edition 2008, College Publications). Conceptual Spaces, Bradford Books, MIT Press, 2000, How Homo Became Sapiens, Oxford University Press, 2003, and The Geometry of Meaning, MIT Press, 2014.pl_PL
dc.referencesBruner, J.S., Goodnow, J.J. and Austin, G.A. (1956). A Study of Thinking. New York: Wiley.pl_PL
dc.referencesChomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.pl_PL
dc.referencesClark, A. (1997). Being there. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.pl_PL
dc.referencesEdelman, G.M. (1987). Neural Darwinism. New York, NY: Basic Books.pl_PL
dc.referencesGedenryd, H. (1998). How Designers Work. Lund: Lund University Cognitive Studies 75.pl_PL
dc.referencesHubel, D.H., Wiesel, T.N. (1968). Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. Journal of Physiology 195: 215–243pl_PL
dc.referencesKosslyn, S. (1980). Image and Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.pl_PL
dc.referencesLakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.pl_PL
dc.referencesLakoff, G., Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.pl_PL
dc.referencesLangacker, R.W. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, vol. 1. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.pl_PL
dc.referencesMarr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.pl_PL
dc.referencesMcCulloch, W.S. and Pitts, W. (1943). A logical calculus of the ideas immanent in nervous activity. Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics 5: 115–133.pl_PL
dc.referencesMiller, G.A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 63: 81–97.pl_PL
dc.referencesNorman, D.A. (1988). The Design of Everyday Things. New York, NY: Basic Books.pl_PL
dc.referencesPutnam, H. (1960). Minds and machines. In: S. Hook (ed.), Dimensions of Mind. New York, NY: New York University Press.pl_PL
dc.referencesRosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 104: 192–233.pl_PL
dc.referencesRumelhart, D.E., McClelland, J.L. (1986). Parallel Distributed Processing, vol. 1–2. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.pl_PL
dc.referencesSearle, J.R. (1980). Minds, brains, and programs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3: 417– 457.pl_PL
dc.referencesShannon, C.E., Weaver, W. (1948). The Mathematical Theory of Communication. Chicago, IL: The University of Illinois Press.pl_PL
dc.referencesShepard, R.N., Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171 (3972): 701–703.pl_PL
dc.referencesTuring, A. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind 59: 433–460.pl_PL
dc.referencesWatson, J.B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviourist views it. Psychological Review 20: 158–177.pl_PL
dc.referencesWeizenbaum, J. (1967). Computer Power and Human Reason. New York, NY: Freeman and Company.pl_PL
dc.identifier.doi10.18778/7969-759-5.02


Pliki tej pozycji

Thumbnail
Thumbnail
Thumbnail

Pozycja umieszczona jest w następujących kolekcjach

Pokaż uproszczony rekord

Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Międzynarodowe
Poza zaznaczonymi wyjątkami, licencja tej pozycji opisana jest jako Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Międzynarodowe